26.01.2013 Views

Presuppositions in Spoken Discourse

Presuppositions in Spoken Discourse

Presuppositions in Spoken Discourse

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Anaphors and Bound <strong>Presuppositions</strong><br />

antecedent on the top of the stack, the use of the anaphoric expression may itself<br />

be trigger<strong>in</strong>g a “pop” of the stack. This work has been discussed extensively as a<br />

computational modal for discourse that can help <strong>in</strong> resolv<strong>in</strong>g anaphors.<br />

Asher & Lascarides (1998b) proposal is the only one I know of that claims<br />

explicitly that hierarchical structure of discourse will also constra<strong>in</strong> the availability<br />

of antecedents for bound presuppositions. In their (1998b) model, as presented <strong>in</strong><br />

section 2.5.2, presuppositions are always bound via a rhetorical relation to another<br />

discourse utterance – this is the attachment site. They argue that only attachment<br />

sites on the right frontier of the discourse structure will be available. In this way<br />

they also believe to constra<strong>in</strong> the b<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g ability of presuppositions <strong>in</strong> a way that<br />

improves upon van der Sandt (1992) by limit<strong>in</strong>g the number of possible<br />

antecedents.<br />

One of the ma<strong>in</strong> arguments for the need to consider discourse structure<br />

hierarchical is evidence from return pops. These are the cases mentioned above<br />

where it seems that speakers return to a focus space or context possibly l<strong>in</strong>early<br />

quite distant from the anaphor but seem<strong>in</strong>gly without process<strong>in</strong>g difficulties. The<br />

idea is that the antecedent is perhaps conceptually closer because the topic or<br />

context where it first appeared is on the right frontier or at the top of the stack<br />

after the pop. The follow<strong>in</strong>g example illustrates a return pop with a personal<br />

pronoun <strong>in</strong> naturally produced dialogue (taken from Fox, 1987, p. 30).<br />

(25) Example of return pop with personal pronoun<br />

Speaker A: Oh, my mother wants to know how’s your grandmother.<br />

Speaker B: Uh, I don’t know. I guess she’s al, she’s alright . She went to the<br />

uh hospital aga<strong>in</strong> today.<br />

Speaker A: Mmm?<br />

Speaker B: And I guess today was the day she’s supposed to f<strong>in</strong>d out if she<br />

goes <strong>in</strong> or not.<br />

Speaker A: Oh. Oh.<br />

Speaker B: Because they’re gonna do the operation on the tear duct first,<br />

before they do the cataracts.<br />

Speaker A: Mm-hm. Right. Yeah.<br />

Speaker B: So I don’t know. I haven’t you know, she wasn’t home by the,<br />

you know, when I left for school today.<br />

Speaker A: Mm hm.<br />

Speaker B: Tch. So uh, I don’t know.<br />

Speaker A. Mmm.<br />

Speaker B: Well my aunt went with here anyway this time.<br />

Speaker A: Mm, hm.<br />

Speaker B: My mother didn’t go.<br />

Speaker A: Mmm.<br />

Speaker B: But uh, I don’t know. She probably has to go <strong>in</strong> soon though.<br />

Here the last she refers to the grandmother, and the analysis given by Fox (1987) is<br />

that it does not even refer to the most recent mention of the grandmother, but the<br />

mention <strong>in</strong> the section marked above the horizontal l<strong>in</strong>e, where the topic is the<br />

91

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!