25.11.2014 Views

Developmental psychology.pdf

Developmental psychology.pdf

Developmental psychology.pdf

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

158 Modes of Awareness<br />

'When I was younger I would<br />

stare for hours at distant things,<br />

pointing my finger directly at things<br />

with both eyes open, and then<br />

close first one eye, then the other.<br />

The object I focused on seemed to<br />

move its position, and I used to<br />

think that there was something<br />

wrong with my eyes.<br />

increases for objects close at hand and decreases when more distant objects are brought<br />

into focus. This adjustment by muscles attached to the eye mechanisms provides the<br />

brain with information about the relative distance of perceived objects.<br />

There are in addition two generally recognized binocular cues, requiring both<br />

eyes, and both are physiological. In convergence, the eyes turn inward as we look at<br />

objects. They do so more for nearby objects than for distant ones, and therefore convergence<br />

is an effective distance cue only for objects within a few meters of the viewer.<br />

These movements prevent blurred vision by placing the image closest to the fovea,<br />

where vision is clearest.<br />

Finally, since the eyes have different locations in the head, each also obtains<br />

a somewhat different view of the same object, as can be experienced by holding a small<br />

object near your face and alternately closing one eye and then the other. The right eye<br />

sees more of the one side whereas the left eye sees more of the other.* This difference,<br />

referred to as retinal disparity, is greater for closer than for distant objects. It too is a<br />

useful distance cue, especially for nearby objects.<br />

Development of Visual Space Cues The behavior of animals and young children<br />

suggests that some of these cues are innate. Animals born with their eyes open and<br />

able to move around soon after birth avoid a cliff without prior experience. Human<br />

beings cannot be tested until they are able to crawl, at the age of six months or so, at<br />

which time they too avoid a cliff, but considerable learning has already occurred (Figure<br />

6.13). Victor, as we saw, had a definite aversion to such situations.<br />

It has been demonstrated that active movement in the environment aids in the<br />

development of space perception. After two groups of kittens were raised in darkness,<br />

one group was given a ride every day in a small carousel, from which their environment<br />

could only be seen. The other group had the same visual experience, but it was achieved<br />

by walking around instead. Afterward, when the kittens were presented with some<br />

problem in space perception, the results were quite clear. The passive kittens, after<br />

riding in the carousel, showed no awareness of a precipice, but none of the walkers<br />

went over the cliff. The visual cues came to have meaning for them through association<br />

with the muscular and tactual experiences (Held & Hein, 1963; Hein, Held, & Gower,<br />

1970).<br />

Figure 6,13<br />

Testing Space Perception. This<br />

apparatus is called a "visual cliff"<br />

because it only presents the<br />

appearance of a cliff. On one side of<br />

the center board the patterned<br />

squares are directly under the glass,<br />

and on the other side they are on the<br />

floor. When human infants are<br />

induced to leave the firm middle<br />

board, almost all of them crawl onto<br />

the "shallow" cliff (Gibson & Walk,<br />

1960).

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!