25.11.2014 Views

Developmental psychology.pdf

Developmental psychology.pdf

Developmental psychology.pdf

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

272 Learning and Information Processing<br />

Sometimes a situation arises in which massed practice, despite its low efficiency,<br />

is necessary. You can be ready for a Spanish exam in 10 hours if two-hour<br />

sessions are used for five consecutive days, but the test is tomorrow and you study day<br />

and night, perhaps for 15 hours. With this massed practice, the required studying time<br />

is 50 percent greater. You are ready for the test on time, but learning by this means,<br />

if not followed by further practice, is also more readily forgotten.<br />

Importance of Effort Practice can be fatiguing and time-consuming. Would it not<br />

be preferable to turn on a tape recorder and learn Spanish while asleep?<br />

Several early studies indicated promising results, and the mass media quickly<br />

publicized these claims. But later investigations showed an important shortcoming in<br />

this work on sleep-learning: the sleepers were partially awakened by the recordings.<br />

Studies designed to correct this defect involved recordings that remained on only when<br />

electroencephalograms showed that the subjects were truly sleeping, as indicated by<br />

various physiological measures. Under these conditions the learners showed no evidence<br />

of learning while asleep (Emmons & Simon,. 1956).<br />

Sleep surely will rise again as a research topic in learning, and it has been of<br />

recent interest in Russia. Some studies there have suggested that sleep-learning is possible,<br />

but close scrutiny of the data has indicated that the learning process is often<br />

commenced prior to falling asleep. The phenomenon in question appears to be memory,<br />

not learning per se, and earlier we saw that verbal learning is best retained when it is<br />

followed by inactivity or sleep. There may even be some consolidation or improvement<br />

of the memory trace during this period (Aarons, 1976).<br />

One general conclusion from all of this research is that the outcome in learning<br />

is typically proportional to the effort. Learning, especially complex learning, requires<br />

concentration. For an objective in the cognitive domain, for example, the value<br />

of recitation versus mere reading has been widely demonstrated. In these studies recitation<br />

does not involve reading aloud but rather actively trying to recall what has been<br />

read. In one instance, students memorized nonsense syllables and historical information<br />

with varying amounts of time spent in each form of learning, including one group<br />

that spent only 20 percent of time reading and 80 percent in recitation. The final outcome<br />

was quite clear; the larger the proportion of time in recitation, the greater was<br />

the learning (Gates, 1917).<br />

The reasons for the advantages of recitation are fairly obvious. Reading with<br />

the knowledge that one must soon recite usually raises motivation; recitation tells how<br />

well one is progressing; and most important, the person who recites usually is preparing<br />

to meet the objective. In learning the geography of southwest Spain, studying a normal<br />

map would be far inferior to using this map and also an outline map with many details<br />

missing. With both maps, learners can study and then intermittently test themselves<br />

on the geographical features.<br />

Dividing the Task The size of the task to be mastered is another persistent issue.<br />

Would it be more efficient to learn this geography as a whole, or would it be best to<br />

break it up into parts, approaching the problem regionally? This issue is called whole<br />

versus part learning, and the findings are specific to the task. In other words, the results<br />

found for one task may or may not apply to another task.<br />

Since geography topics can be readily divided, the part approach would have<br />

special merit for southwest Spain, but putting the parts together sometimes requires<br />

much additional work. Language, for example, is often learned in parts, as one works<br />

on vocabulary, the indirect object, then the subjunctive, and so forth, but speaking a<br />

foreign language fluently is not so easy. In most cases it seems best to use a flexible<br />

plan, starting with the whole but subdividing it as necessary, when difficult aspects<br />

require special attention (Woodworth & Schlosberg, 1954).

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!