25.11.2014 Views

Developmental psychology.pdf

Developmental psychology.pdf

Developmental psychology.pdf

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

196 Learning and Information Processing<br />

Figure 7.18<br />

Reciprocal Conditioning. The cats'<br />

behavior influenced the monk, just as<br />

the monk's behavior influenced the<br />

cats'.<br />

1 ^^ ~^<br />

cats appear<br />

S,<br />

monk coughs<br />

Monk's Behavior<br />

Cats' Behavior<br />

Extensions of Conditioning Principles<br />

In closing, we should make one final observation concerning the monk and the monastery<br />

cats. Through the principles of conditioning, the monk controlled the cats and<br />

thereby ate in peace. But according to operant conditioning, the cats, in turn, also<br />

controlled him.<br />

'Because I would do almost<br />

anything to avoid hurting my<br />

father's feelings, I taught myself at<br />

an early age to laugh, or at least<br />

smile, at his poor jokes. Each time I<br />

laughed, however, I was increasing<br />

the likelihood of his telling another<br />

joke.<br />

Conditioning as Reciprocal Operant conditioning often involves mutual<br />

reinforcement. The response of one member supports the response of the other. This<br />

situation is called reciprocal conditioning, for each person's behavior is a reinforcing<br />

stimulus for another individual. The monk kept the cats running, but they kept him<br />

coughing.<br />

If we analyze this situation according to the components of operant conditioning,<br />

the discriminative stimulus for the monk was the approach of the cats; his<br />

response was to cough; and his reinforcement was that the cats ran away, leaving him<br />

to eat his meal in peace. For the hungry cats, creeping toward a midday snack, the<br />

discriminative stimulus was the monk's cough; their response was to run away; and<br />

their reinforcement was to be removed from an anxiety-producing situation (Figure<br />

7.18).<br />

In operant conditioning an individual operates on his or her environment, and<br />

the environment, which includes the living organisms in it, operates on the individual<br />

as well. The parent controls the child, but as any parent knows, the child also controls<br />

the parent.* A government creates and amends laws to control the people, but it does<br />

so according to how well the people respond to them. There are exceptions to this rule—<br />

including prisoners, the very elderly, and emotionally disturbed persons, who sometimes<br />

have no control over their caretakers—but according to Skinner and his followers,<br />

it applies in a broad way to most interpersonal relationships.<br />

Conditioning and Thinking The monk, one must admit, also thought up this whole<br />

scheme for training the animals in the first place. How do we understand this behavior<br />

in terms of conditioning?<br />

As a rule, psychologists interested in conditioning are not concerned with<br />

thinking. They study overt responses instead, focusing on the monk's finding the sack,<br />

catching the cats, and so forth. No psychologist can study everything, and those concerned<br />

with conditioning prefer to limit themselves in this way, utilizing the concepts<br />

of reinforcement, shaping, and chaining when referring to complex behavior. If the<br />

monk had employed this same scheme earlier, or even parts of it, its reappearance in<br />

this instance certainly would be explained on the basis of the reinforcement principle.<br />

One radical approach to conditioning earlier in the history of <strong>psychology</strong> described<br />

thinking as subvocal speech. Thinking, just like any other human activity, was<br />

assumed to be based in some bodily reactions. The problem was that these reactions<br />

could not be observed directly, and therefore thinking was considered beyond the realm<br />

of appropriate scientific inquiry (Watson, 1925).<br />

Today there is within the conditioning approach greater recognition of internal<br />

or mental processes. Our thoughts are sometimes regarded as responses and the<br />

imagined outcomes are considered reinforcement. Each new thought, or response, is<br />

thus a reinforcement for the preceding one and a cue for the next one. The monk produced<br />

internal chains in this fashion until he discovered the solution. Then he put the<br />

solution into practice in an overt manner.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!