25.11.2014 Views

Developmental psychology.pdf

Developmental psychology.pdf

Developmental psychology.pdf

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

276 Learning and Information Processing<br />

was found that most subjects preferred to work for the reward, although this preference<br />

diminished in the older group (Tarte, 1981). Such differences among individuals<br />

may reflect differences in the socialization process, which certainly can influence how<br />

things are perceived (Wilson, 1982; Tzuriel & Heywood, 1984).<br />

"The parents of a nigh school<br />

friend used punishment in an<br />

attempt to influence their son's<br />

actions. For example, it was not<br />

uncommon for them to spend a<br />

night at home drinking or to drink<br />

when they went out, but when<br />

Steve began to drink with his<br />

friends, they became very upset<br />

and immediately put him on at least<br />

one month's restriction.<br />

Steve learned to dislike his<br />

parents extremely for their<br />

punishment, which was often<br />

severe, especially when he saw<br />

them doing the same thing. His<br />

drinking stopped for a month, but<br />

as soon as the month was over, he<br />

was back at it again. Soon he had<br />

to get drunk every weekend, and<br />

when he tried marijuana he had to<br />

be stoned every day. Me became<br />

very distant from his parents and<br />

no longer told them his feelings on<br />

anything. As he began to take more<br />

drugs, the slightest reprimand from<br />

them would send him running from<br />

the house, sometimes not to return<br />

for days or weeks.<br />

Now they say that Steve is<br />

severely disturbed. I don't know<br />

. . . but I believe that his parents"<br />

form of punishment was one of the<br />

influencing factors in his problems<br />

today.<br />

Contrast Effect The importance of perception or expectation has been demonstrated<br />

even in animals. Suppose, for example, that two groups of rats learn to solve a maze<br />

for rewards of different magnitudes. One group receives a small amount of food; the<br />

other receives a large meal. The significance of these rewards will be readily evident,<br />

for the large-reward subjects will learn faster than the others.<br />

But what happens when the size of the reward is shifted? After almost two<br />

dozen trials in one early experiment, the rats receiving the large reward were given a<br />

lesser amount, equal to the amount received by the other group. With this change these<br />

rats immediately altered their performance markedly. Their speed of mazerunning<br />

became significantly lower than that of the other group, even though the two<br />

sets of subjects were now receiving the same reward (Crespi, 1942).<br />

Toads behaved differently when a water reward was used. The large-reward<br />

group performed best initially, as anticipated, and after 50 or 80 trials this group was<br />

given the same amount of water as the small-reward group. Their performance then<br />

declined to just the same level as the other group. Contrary to expectations, there was<br />

no further decrement below the small-reward group (Schmajuk, Segura, & Ruidiaz,<br />

1981).<br />

In other words, no contrast effect appeared. A contrast effect means that the<br />

value of an incentive changes according to the incentive with which it is compared.<br />

After a large reward, a small reward seems much smaller than it would otherwise.<br />

Similarly, after a small reward, a large reward appears much larger than is normally<br />

the case.<br />

This phenomenon is widespread among human beings, who are always thinking<br />

things over, ruminating on the past, and anticipating the future. If a man wins a<br />

million pesetas playing the Spanish lottery, subsequent attempts that yield 200 pesetas<br />

seem hardly worth the effort. Expectations can influence our reactions to any incentive,<br />

even though the ways in which they vary with different rewards and different species<br />

are not yet fully understood.<br />

Negative Effects of Punishment Regardless of expectations, there is one clear<br />

difference between reward and punishment. Reward, if improperly used, generally<br />

produces no more than disinterest or apathy. The incentive looks like a bribe or seems<br />

too small, and the subject does not respond. But when punishment is used improperly,<br />

it can lead to diverse negative outcomes.<br />

In general, the punished person learns to dislike the punitive agent and also<br />

the activity with which the punishment is associated. Such negative attitudes are highly<br />

undesirable, yet they can remain for long periods, even through the individual's lifetime.*<br />

Punishment, especially when coupled with conflict, also can produce diverse<br />

forms of mental disturbance, including anxiety, lethargy, hyperactivity, and uselessly<br />

repetitive behavior. Animals have shown these reactions following punishment in experimental<br />

situations, and clinical observations of severely punished children provide<br />

further evidence (Hill, 1973).<br />

Punishment may even sustain the very behavior against which it is used, for<br />

the effectiveness of punishment depends partly on whether the undesired behavior was<br />

established under punitive or rewarding conditions. If a person's poor performance at<br />

the bank is punished by a salary decrement and loss of vacation, she will develop a<br />

negative attitude toward that institution. This attitude will result in still poorer performance,<br />

for which there is further punishment. Punishment then serves to reinforce<br />

the negative attitude toward the bank, and an escalating negative attitude has been<br />

established (Tarpy, 1975).

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!