25.11.2014 Views

Developmental psychology.pdf

Developmental psychology.pdf

Developmental psychology.pdf

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

50 Background and Methods<br />

RESEARCH IN PERSPECTIVE<br />

In concluding this overview of research methods, we return to the original research<br />

question: "What happens when we dream?" As the reader now understands, dreaming<br />

is a complex activity and this research question is indeed a broad one. Given the limitations<br />

of time, space, and energy, it must be studied in parts, each in its own detail.<br />

One might ask instead: "What happens physiologically when we dream?" But<br />

here again there are too many possibilities; innumerable changes could be occurring<br />

throughout the body. One might ask: "What changes take place in the brain during<br />

dreaming?" To be still more specific, and to develop a highly appropriate topic for<br />

research, one might ask: "What changes, if any, occur in electrical brain activity during<br />

sleep?" In short, most investigators concern themselves with very specific questions<br />

and use very specific research methods, singly or in various combinations.<br />

Through use of the electroencephalograph, which provides a record of some<br />

brain activities, we now know that certain types of brain waves, those of low voltage<br />

and high frequency, characterize dreaming. These waves are more like those of a person<br />

who is awake rather than asleep, but because it is difficult to awaken a sleeper<br />

showing this brain-wave pattern, the REM or dream state is also known as paradoxical<br />

sleep. Further research, probing beneath the brain's surface, has suggested that the<br />

neural bases for these activities originate in part in the brain area known as the reticular<br />

formation (Hobson & McCarley, 1977).<br />

Inference and Theory<br />

Through naturalistic observation we have learned that behavioral dimensions of<br />

dreaming exist, evident not only in the rapid eye movements but also in the changes<br />

of the facial muscles. Using these behavioral and physiological measures, and waking<br />

people at specified moments, we now know that nearly everyone dreams, that there are<br />

predictable topics for most of our dreams, that dreams sometimes occur in non-REM<br />

sleep, and that we have several dreams per night. Babies are in REM sleep almost onethird<br />

of their lives, whereas adults spend one-twelfth of their lives dreaming (Figure<br />

2.12).<br />

Are babies actually dreaming during REM sleep? Do they experience the visual-auditory<br />

images that adults call dreaming, even in the early weeks of life? It seems<br />

possible that babies undergo some rudimentary form of dreaming for, in addition to<br />

the REMs, they show the other physiological and behavioral accompaniments of<br />

dreaming. We cannot be certain, and therefore the study of dreams, just as other topics<br />

in <strong>psychology</strong>, often requires that the investigator go beyond the demonstrated facts.<br />

Figure 2.12.<br />

Age and Dreaming. As people grow<br />

older they spend less time sleeping<br />

and a decreasing proportion of their<br />

sleep time in the RBM state. Hence,<br />

they presumably do much less<br />

dreaming (Hartmann, 1967).<br />

Role of Inference Suppose that the young woman who reported the dreams at the<br />

beginning of this chapter is observed in bed. She might be sleeping, feigning sleep,<br />

resting, unconscious, dreaming, or even dead. The only evidence immediately available<br />

is that she is in bed, and on this basis alone we cannot be sure of her condition or what<br />

she is doing.<br />

But we can make an inference. An inference, as we saw earlier, is an idea or<br />

judgment drawn from some observable events. It concerns something not directly evident<br />

but is based on available data. As we approach the woman, we notice that her<br />

chest is rising and falling. These events are not consistent with being dead. Accordingly,<br />

the inference that the woman has died is no longer tenable.<br />

We see that her breathing is deep, not shallow, so we make another inference.<br />

She is not unconscious. The woman's eyes are closed, and occasionally she mumbles<br />

or seems to be talking to someone who is not in the room. Hence we tentatively discard<br />

the inference that she is resting. Our observations have led instead to the inference<br />

that the woman is sleeping and perhaps dreaming.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!