25.11.2014 Views

Developmental psychology.pdf

Developmental psychology.pdf

Developmental psychology.pdf

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Perception and Consciousness 161<br />

One explanation suggested that it was due to kinesthetic sensations in the eyes<br />

as they looked from one figure to the other. These muscle movements somehow produced<br />

the phi phenomenon. But then a demonstration was set up in such a way that<br />

eye movements could not possibly account for the outcome. Two pairs of figures were<br />

used, one above the other, and the perceived movement was left to right in one instance<br />

and right to left in the other. Since the eye movements could not occur in two opposite<br />

directions at the same time, they could not be responsible for the illusion.<br />

The phi phenomenon apparently is dependent on some fixed reaction of the<br />

visual system to these particular stimulus relationships. It may ensure that we perceive<br />

the motion in very fast-moving objects (Rock, 1975).<br />

We all experience this illusion in motion pictures, which takes place as follows.<br />

After an image appears, a shutter blocks all projected light, and a new frame with a<br />

slightly different image moves into place. The shutter opens; this still image is projected<br />

onto the screen; then it is blocked; and another slightly different image takes its<br />

place. The whole sequence is repeated again and again, without any objective movement<br />

on the screen, which would only be seen as a blur. Instead, the motion picture is<br />

a dramatic example of the phi phenomenon.<br />

Illusion of Distance Illusions involving distance and shape are even more common<br />

than illusions of movement, and one group of investigators, believing that learning is<br />

important, formulated the carpentered-world hypothesis. This hypothesis states that<br />

people who live among rectangular doors, tables, walls, platforms, and benches develop<br />

certain expectancies from this experience, making them especially susceptible to illusions<br />

involving these patterns. The Zulus, in contrast, spend much time outdoors and recognize<br />

the circle as the dominant pattern. When the Zulus were compared with some residents<br />

of Illinois, the issue was the role of learning, and significant differences between the<br />

two groups were found. The Americans were much more prone to illusions of distances<br />

involving straight lines and rectangular geometric patterns (Herskovits, Campbell, &<br />

Segall, 1969; Figure 6.14).<br />

Altogether, 15 cultures were tested, and since susceptibility to this illusion was<br />

found in all instances, biological factors are clearly involved to some degree. But the<br />

large group differences indicate that experience is an augmenting factor. In fact, the<br />

chief thrust of this chapter so far is that human perception, although dependent on<br />

inherited sense organs and neural reactions, is something more than a simple reception<br />

of information out there in the world around us. To this stimulation, we add the influence<br />

of earlier learning.<br />

Figure 6.14<br />

Parallelogram Illusion. The AC<br />

diagonal appears about the same<br />

length as ВС, but it is 16 percent<br />

shorter. If the parallelogram actually<br />

were a table top seen from one side<br />

and slightly above, the left-hand<br />

diagonal would represent a greater<br />

distance across the surface than the<br />

right-hand diagonal. Hence it is<br />

judged longer by people who live in a<br />

"carpentered world" but not by the<br />

Zulu of South Africa, who have<br />

circular huts, circular cattle pens, and<br />

other circular settings.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!