17.01.2013 Views

Chapter 2. Prehension

Chapter 2. Prehension

Chapter 2. Prehension

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

<strong>Chapter</strong> 5 - Movement Before Contact 119<br />

Jeannerod, 1988). The reaching movements are inaccurate and much<br />

slower, having lower peak velocities and longer deceleration phases.<br />

Thus, the posterior parietal cortex is implicated in disruption of the<br />

convergence of visual and proprioceptive maps for visually guided<br />

reaching.<br />

Other evidence has been observed in behavioral pointing studies<br />

for a mapping between extrinsic and intrinsic coordinate frames (step<br />

2). In Soechting and Terzuolo (1990), subjects were asked to remem-<br />

ber a target’s location, and then asked to point to that remembered spot<br />

in the dark. The distance and direction of the target were the extrinsic<br />

measures while intrinsic measures were the shoulder and elbow joint<br />

angles in terms of angular elevation and yaw. Subjects were able to<br />

point in the correct direction but there were errors in the radial distance<br />

from the shoulder, ranging from 6.7 to 11.7 cm average root mean<br />

square total error in each subject. Soechting and Terzuolo argued that<br />

the errors were due to the transformation between extrinsic and<br />

intrinsic coordinates, and suggested that the problem is solved only<br />

approximately, thus leading to predictable distortions. Soechting and<br />

Flanders (1991) showed that for pointing tasks, a spatial location is<br />

first transformed from head centered to shoulder centered spatial<br />

coordinates. The location is then redefined in an intrinsic kinematic<br />

framework by two separate channels that convert target azimuth<br />

(horizontal direction) into intended yaw angles of arm in sagittal plane<br />

when the hand is at the target, and target elevation and distance into<br />

arm segment elevation angles. The difference between current and<br />

intended arm angles is then transformed into a motor command<br />

signaling direction of hand movement. The error patterns suggest that<br />

the CNS uses linear approximations of the exact nonlinear relation<br />

between limb segment angles and target location7.<br />

Separate encoding of distance and direction was obtained in motor<br />

memory research in the 1970s, using linear or lever positioning tasks.<br />

In such tasks, blindfolded humans grasped a handle using palm, pad<br />

or side opposition, and moved to a preselected or experimenter-<br />

defined endpoint. Then, either immediately or after some filled or<br />

unfilled retention interval, the individuals were asked to reproduce the<br />

distance or location, from a new or different starting location.<br />

Evidence supported a qualitatively different memory for location than<br />

distance (subjects are better at reproducing locations than distances),<br />

and that there were different patterns of forgetting of location and dis-<br />

7Separate planning channels may also exist for direction, amplitude and force<br />

(Favilla et al., 1989; Riehle & Requin, 1989; Rosenbaum, 1980).

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!