17.01.2013 Views

Chapter 2. Prehension

Chapter 2. Prehension

Chapter 2. Prehension

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

<strong>Chapter</strong> 5 - Movement Before Contact 123<br />

in the motor cortex, providing evidence that the motor cortex may be<br />

viewed as a computational map. At the same time, some features of<br />

limb movements may not be explicitly planned by supraspinal sys-<br />

tems. It is possible that the CNS is using simplifying rules to generate<br />

close but inexact approximations (or ballparks) of an intended mov-<br />

ment. The hand path, for example, which tends to be in straight lines<br />

and exhibit bell-shaped velocity profiles, may not be being planned,<br />

but may emerge as a result of the CNS transformations from goal lo-<br />

cation to intrinsic joint angles or muscle lengths and/or from the me-<br />

chanical properties of the limb.<br />

5.3.2 Symmetric vs asymmetric velocity profiles<br />

A pointing study performed by Atkeson and Hollerbach (1985)<br />

brought out important features about unrestrained arm movements.<br />

Subjects started with their hand in one of six locations in space, and<br />

were asked to move their arm within a plane to another location which<br />

was lit with a light-emitting diode. In order to collect these data,<br />

Atkeson and Hollerbach used a SELSPOT system, allowing them to<br />

track the subject’s index finger tip, wrist, elbow, and shoulder move-<br />

ments over time. Small, light-emitting diodes were attached to the<br />

body part to be tracked, and solid state cameras captured the kinemat-<br />

ics of the movement. In terms of the path of the wrist, some paths<br />

were straight-line, some were curved, depending on the location of the<br />

movement in the workspace. Subjects were required to carry a load in<br />

their hands for some of the trials. For a given direction, there was<br />

path invariance with speed and load. Atkeson and Hollerbach found<br />

that practice did not influence the movements. They showed that the<br />

tangential velocity profile, scaled both in time and amplitude, was<br />

invariant for all subjects, speeds, loads and directions (shown in<br />

Figure 5.4).<br />

Other investigators have not always found symmetric velocity<br />

profiles in pointing tasks. MacKenzie, Marteniuk, Dugas, Liske, and<br />

Eickmeier (1987) replicated the conditions from Paul Fitts’ study on<br />

discrete aiming (Fitts 1954, Experiment 2), and found systematic<br />

effects of target size on the degree of asymmetry in the tangential<br />

velocity profiles. Subjects were asked to point with a stylus ‘as<br />

quickly and as accurately as possible’ to a target of varying size and at<br />

varying distances. The question being asked concerned whether there<br />

was a reliable kinematic measure of the precision requirements of the<br />

task. In this case, Fitts’ Law (Fitts, 1954) was used, which states

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!