17.01.2013 Views

Chapter 2. Prehension

Chapter 2. Prehension

Chapter 2. Prehension

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

70 THE PHASES OF PREHENSION<br />

(right side of Figure 3.3). The details, as well as the variations that<br />

might occur with each grasping instance, are present within each<br />

schema. The only invariances suggested in Arbib’s model are the<br />

triggering of the schemas.<br />

4.<strong>2.</strong>3 Neural task plans<br />

In a typical experiment to study movement latencies, Jeannerod<br />

and Biguer (1982) had subjects move as “fast and accurate” as<br />

possible to reach and grasp an object placed in a specific location, in<br />

response to a visual go signal. They measured arm and hand muscle<br />

activation as subjects reached for and grasped an object. Recording<br />

the activity level of electromyography (EMG) data, Jeannerod and<br />

Biguer noted a 250 ms lag time from the ‘GO’ signal to a burst in<br />

EMG activity in the biceps muscle of the arm (see Appendix A).<br />

About 20 ms later, an EMG burst was seen in the extensor digitorum<br />

communis, the muscle used to extend the fingers as the hand opened<br />

from the initial resting posture of thumb and index finger pads<br />

touching. Actual movement of the fingers occurred about 80 to 100<br />

ms later, which is the electromechanical delay time for isometric<br />

tension to be established in the muscle.<br />

While Jeannerod and Biguer noted a 250 ms lag time, the time lag<br />

that it takes for the subject to initiate movement is not fixed. This time<br />

to react after a stimulus, or reaction time, can vary depending on a<br />

myriad of factors including the difficulty of the task. More complex<br />

tasks generally increase the reaction time of the subject. For simple<br />

movements, the reaction time is shorter than for more complex<br />

movements. For example, in a classic study done by Henry and<br />

Rogers (1960), subjects were asked, in response to an auditory “go”<br />

signal, to make either a simple movement (lift finger from key), a<br />

complex movement (lift finger, reach and grasp suspended ball), or a<br />

more complex movement (lift finger, reach forward and strike<br />

suspended ball with back of hand, reverse direction to push button,<br />

and then strike another suspended ball). For adult subjects, mean<br />

reaction time increased as the movement complexity increased, from<br />

158 ms for the simple finger lift task to 197 ms for grasping to 213 ms<br />

for the more complex movement sequence requiring ball strikes and<br />

changes in direction. More recently, reaction times to initiate grasps<br />

have been unaffected by systematic manipulations of object texture<br />

(Fikes, Klatzky & Lederman, 1993) or orientation (Stelmach,<br />

Castiello & Jeannerod, 1993). Thus time to intiate a grasp depends on<br />

the sensory modality for processing the go signal (auditory or visual),

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!