17.01.2013 Views

Chapter 2. Prehension

Chapter 2. Prehension

Chapter 2. Prehension

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

<strong>Chapter</strong> 9 - Reevaluation and Future Directions 343<br />

Experiments also suggest computational models. Effective<br />

experiments have included perturbation studies, where microslips or<br />

externally imposed changes in object properties are observed. Models<br />

are needed for a better understanding of the methods in which the<br />

computations may be performed. Analytic models of friction, and<br />

levels of asperities in the contacting surfaces are needed, as are<br />

computational models of force generation by the muscle, skin, and<br />

‘passive’ viscoelastic properties of the hand. The weighting and<br />

calibration of information across receptor types and sensory systems<br />

is needed to understand better how exteroceptive and proprioceptive<br />

systems are integrated with descending motor commands.<br />

Computational and experimental models show the methods by<br />

which feedback and feedforward control can work, depending on the<br />

success of anticipatory movements. The role of practice and past<br />

experience needs to be modelled for better understanding the role of<br />

feedforward processes. If unexpected perturbations occur, feedback<br />

control can make fine adjustments. At which levels feedforward and<br />

fine adjustments can be implemented need to be defined more clearly.<br />

Making explicit the influence of various constraints on the<br />

environment/performer, the need for a common vocabulary between<br />

experimentalists and roboticists is paramount. Much can be gained<br />

through increased sharing and communication, using a triangular<br />

approach to science.<br />

This book is not comprehensive; many topics were left untouched.<br />

Handedness, asymmetries (in hand and brain) and bimanual activities<br />

(splitting the task demands) are glaring in their omission. Illusions<br />

like the size-weight illusion were only alluded to. Postural (trunk,<br />

hip, and lower limb) involvements were excluded from consideration,<br />

as were parallels between locomotion and limb control. Little was<br />

said about central and peripheral fatigue, and corresponding<br />

adaptations. Detailed treatment of the research on special populations<br />

has been left out. These include individuals with sensory or<br />

neurological dysfunction, such as blindness, and patients with brain<br />

lesions (e.g., apraxia, optic and motor ataxia). The effects of other<br />

diseases (such as arthritis), conditions (like congenital anhydrosis)<br />

and injuries (dislocations, carpal tunnel syndrome) on hand function,<br />

are also notably absent.<br />

It is our goal that this text be relevant to workers in the applied<br />

sciences. There is much relevance here to human factors applications.<br />

This includes the design of human-computer interfaces, other human-<br />

machine interfaces (e.g., automobile control panels), hand tools, and<br />

remote manipulators. For computer graphics, there are issues for

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!