17.01.2013 Views

Chapter 2. Prehension

Chapter 2. Prehension

Chapter 2. Prehension

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

156 THE PHASES OF PREHENSION<br />

creased slope and longer duration prior to lift for heavier objects<br />

(Johansson & Westling, 1988b), to be discussed in <strong>Chapter</strong> 6.<br />

A similar experiment was conducted to examine the effects of ob-<br />

ject texture, which can be partially assessed by vision (Weir, Mac-<br />

Kenzie, Marteniuk, Cargoe & Fraser, 1991). Subjects reached,<br />

grasped and lifted slippery (coated with Vaseline), neutral (polished<br />

metal) or coarse (covered in sandpaper) dowels in blocked conditions.<br />

Results indicated that a longer time was spent in contact with the<br />

slippery dowel, prior to lift, compared with the other two dowels.<br />

They suggested that even though the texture information was available<br />

during vision, the kinematics prior to contact revealed no anticipation<br />

appropriate for the coefficient of friction at contact. Replicating and<br />

extending this study, Fikes, Klatzky and Lederman (1993) showed<br />

that under randomized conditions, reaching for a slippery dowel,<br />

individuals spent a greater time from movement onset to contact. They<br />

discussed these results in terms of the greater geometric and dynamic<br />

precision required for lifting a slippery object. Given the lower<br />

coefficient of friction, subjects might adapt to the frictional<br />

requirements in two ways: post contact (as demonstrated by Johan-<br />

sson and Westling, 1984b) or precontact. Fikes et al. suggest that they<br />

may prepare appropriate grip geometry precontact, and the associated<br />

precision for placement anticipating slip might take more time. This is<br />

an important suggestion, because such placement precision demands<br />

should be quantifiable in kinematic landmarks prior to contact.<br />

In this section, we have seen how intrinsic object properties like<br />

size and weight, and extrinsic properties like distance, direction, object<br />

motion and surface texture affect kinematic landmarks of the reaching<br />

and grasping components. In examining task requirements, the evi-<br />

dence seems to indicate that reaching with the arm to point is very dif-<br />

ferent from reaching to grasp, and further that the requirements for the<br />

hand are ‘driving the arm’. The kinematics of reaching and grasping<br />

reflect the setting up of opposition space parameters, and the anticipa-<br />

tory preparation for task demands once the object is grasped. The arm<br />

and the hand seem to be functionally coupled in their control.<br />

5.4.2 Visual and mechanical perturbations to grasping<br />

Additional evidence for the view that the arm and hand are func-<br />

tionally coupled to align the grasping surface patches on the hand with<br />

the opposition vector is derived from perturbation studies. We con-<br />

sider mechanical perturbation studies, where unexpected loads are<br />

applied to oppose or interfere with the ongoing movement (as in being

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!