17.01.2013 Views

Chapter 2. Prehension

Chapter 2. Prehension

Chapter 2. Prehension

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

122 THE PHASES OF PREHENSION<br />

where F is force, K is stiffness of the spring, and ax is the change in<br />

displacement that occurs when the force is applied. The agonist and<br />

antagonist muscles can be viewed as a pair of opposing springs whose<br />

equilibrium states determines the joint angle. Movement occurs by<br />

varying the equilibrium point over time, changing the relative tensions<br />

of the two opposing springs. An equilibrium configuration is defined<br />

for a given value of muscle activation as that position where the forces<br />

of opposing muscles generate equal and opposite torques about the<br />

joints (Hogan 1985). Once the equilbrium point is set, an invariant<br />

characteristic curve is established, and movement occurs to bring the<br />

muscle into the new equilbrium point. The trajectory of the limb fol-<br />

lows an invariant path that brings the limb quickly into equilibrium,<br />

and no specific dynamics computation is made then by the CNS. An<br />

advantage of this method of control is that the muscles-as-springs au-<br />

tomatically generate restoring forces to keep the arm on a specified<br />

virtual trajectory when small perturbations occur, avoiding the need<br />

for an active feedback controller to correct small errors. The issue of<br />

motor equivalence is simplified, as is the degrees of freedom problem,<br />

and variability, since speed is controlled independently of resting<br />

length.<br />

For the mass-spring model, a controller is not needed for generat-<br />

ing joint torques over time (Step 3 in Figure 5.3) because they can be<br />

generated by properties of the muscles themselves. The controller<br />

changes the virtual position of the arm (i.e., the equilibrium point<br />

between agonist/antagonist muscles) by adjusting some control<br />

parameter. Feldman (1986) argued that the resting length threshold<br />

between agonist/antagonist is selected as the control variable. In<br />

contrast, Bizzi (1980) argued that the equilibrium point is controlled<br />

by setting the alpha motoneuron activity to the agonisdantagonist<br />

muscles. There is evidence for both. For example, Bizzi, Dev,<br />

Morasso and Polit (1978), trained primates to perform pointing tasks.<br />

After training, they were deafferented. Under conditions where<br />

unknown loads were added to the arm, there was consistent over or<br />

under shooting. When the load was removed, the correct position was<br />

attained.<br />

In summary, kinematic transformations have been observed in be-<br />

havioral data that suggests that the CNS uses linear approximations for<br />

performing the transformation. There is little variability in the angular<br />

motion between the shoulder and elbow joints (Soechting &<br />

Lacquaniti, 1981). A directional population vector has been observed<br />

*A system is in equilibrium when there are no net forces and torques acting on it.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!