17.01.2013 Views

Chapter 2. Prehension

Chapter 2. Prehension

Chapter 2. Prehension

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

<strong>Chapter</strong> 4 - Planning of <strong>Prehension</strong> 77<br />

measured while the posture was maintained for a few seconds. In this<br />

pad opposition task, mean grip size correlated positively with object<br />

size. Error in matching was computed for each subject as the<br />

difference between the grip size used during normal grasping and the<br />

person’s measured grip size. While object size estimation was shown<br />

to be linearly related to the actual object size, errors ranged between<br />

f1.5 cm with very little consistency (of the six subjects, two subjects<br />

always underestimated while the rest tended to overestimated). This<br />

experiment demonstrates the ability of the CNS to plan an opposition<br />

space, but with qualifications. The visual perception of object<br />

properties is an estimation (visual scaling) which is shown to be<br />

highly accurate. Transforming this visual information into motor<br />

commands (visuomotor scaling) causes errors, which Jeannerod and<br />

Decety argue could stem from the change from one coordinate system<br />

to another. Visual objects are encoded in retinal coordinates, whereas<br />

hand configurations are encoded in some other frame. Retinal cues<br />

related to object size are not sufficient for determining grip size,<br />

particularly in a precision task using pad opposition. They suggest<br />

that for accuracy, visual feedback is needed to improve the planned<br />

hand posture for correcting visuomotor biases and reducing motor<br />

variability.<br />

In a similar experiment, Chan, Carello, and Turvey (1990) asked<br />

subjects to use their other hand to evaluate the size of the object. An<br />

object was placed in front of the subject, who then used the thumb and<br />

index finger of the left hand to indicate the size of the object. Subjects<br />

were able to do this accurately in a linear fashion for objects below a<br />

10 cm width. Above that, there were non-linearities due to a ceiling<br />

effect; that is, there were biomechanical constraints on the hand.<br />

Using the thumb and middle finger removed these non-linearities.<br />

The speed with which object properties are perceived is important.<br />

Klatzky et al. (1987, 1990) analyzed the two-dimensional perception<br />

of objects, arguing that the projection of a three dimensional object on<br />

the retina can be used for making decisions about interacting with the<br />

object. By varying the area and depth of the two-dimensional<br />

projection of an object, they showed an effect on the chosen grasp.<br />

As seen in Figure 4.6, subjects pinched small things and clenched<br />

larger things. Since object size is a critical planning parameter, it is<br />

noteworthy that a simple two-dimensional representation of the object<br />

can be used for choosing a grasp posture. This representation is<br />

available at the level of the retina, thus quickly accessible to the CNS.<br />

Intrinsic object properties can be perturbed at movement onset in<br />

order to observe how long it takes to perceive object properties, or at

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!