17.01.2013 Views

Chapter 2. Prehension

Chapter 2. Prehension

Chapter 2. Prehension

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

<strong>Chapter</strong> 7 - Opposition Space Phases 287<br />

7.1 Planning an Opposition Space<br />

In order to perform the simple task of lifting an object such as a<br />

hammer, preparatory processes related to the organization and<br />

planning of the upcoming movement occur in the CNS, coordinating<br />

such a highly complex activity. In our model, this first step in the task<br />

plan involves selecting an opposition space useful for the task. This<br />

involves three aspects:<br />

1) perceiving task-specific object properties,<br />

2) selecting a grasp strategy, and<br />

3) planning a hand location and orientation.<br />

The choice of an opposition space depends on information<br />

perceived about an object, including extrinsic and intrinsic properties.<br />

The particular properties perceived are task-related. For example,<br />

color is not particularly useful for grasping, unless we wish to select<br />

objects only of a particular color, i.e., other than helping to distinguish<br />

color as a feature of the object. Experimental evidence suggests that<br />

intrinsic object properties, such as size and shape and surface spatial<br />

density, and extrinsic object properties, such as object location and<br />

orientation, are perceived prior to movement. Weight is estimated,<br />

based on size/weight relationships. The particular properties are also<br />

perceived in a hand related manner. Surfaces discounted for grasping<br />

include inaccessible surfaces, surfaces too wide for the hand’s span,<br />

and surfaces too narrow for the hand’s width. Part of the choosing of<br />

an opposition space entails seeing an opposition vector which has a<br />

hand-sized magnitude between two hand-sized surfaces that will<br />

satisfy the required degrees of freedom of the task. Figure 7.3<br />

summarizes this notion, showing the opposition vector and its<br />

properties. The opposition vector has an orientation with respect to an<br />

approach vector.<br />

The choice of an opposition space also depends on previous<br />

knowledge about the behavior of objects that our brains have collected<br />

over time (phylogenetically and ontogenetically), and our ability to<br />

predict and anticipate task-specific prehensile occurrences. For<br />

example, the thumb and index finger do not precisely have to grasp the<br />

object at the same time. One can anticipate how to ‘push’ the object<br />

into the grasp. This knowledge includes anticipation of object rotations<br />

and translations in relationship to where fingers are placed relative to<br />

the object’s center of mass. In addition, knowledge about the cone of<br />

friction is necessary for anticipating where to grasp and how to

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!