17.01.2013 Views

Chapter 2. Prehension

Chapter 2. Prehension

Chapter 2. Prehension

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

58 THE PHASES OF PREHENSION<br />

untary but depends on the person’s intent. If one chooses to ignore<br />

the fragility of the object, there will be no response.<br />

The power of conceptual models like Arbib’s, Paillard’s, and<br />

Greene’s is that they provide a summary of what is known and a<br />

starting framework for detailing the planning and control processes.<br />

Their validity can be argued from the fact that they are based on exper-<br />

imental evidence. They rely on perceptual and motor units all working<br />

together, not under one central controller, but under distributed con-<br />

trol. They also represent plausible views which in turn suggest<br />

testable hypotheses about central nervous system (CNS) mechanisms,<br />

planning and control algorithms for such a complex behavior as<br />

reaching and grasping.<br />

Conceptual models are useful as a starting ground for understand-<br />

ing the complex interaction between a performer and the environment.<br />

Sensory information is needed at crucial times for completion of the<br />

motor task. The CNS, however, being highly parallel and redundant,<br />

can solve problems in many ways. It exhibits motor equivalence, in<br />

that motor commands are nonspecific and different spatial- temporal<br />

patterns of action can produce the same outcome. Muscles can vary<br />

their functional roles from movement to movement. It has been<br />

shown that handwriting, for example, is the same regardless of the<br />

writing implement, speed, size, or limb used. Another issue is the<br />

availability of sensory information. If available from a given modal-<br />

ity, it has the potential of being used in the control of the movement; if<br />

not, other modalities of sensory information will be used. If visual in-<br />

formation is available, it will be used, causing movements to be<br />

slower. If not, subjects rely more on touch. Finally, an important is-<br />

sue in studying human motor control is the fact that there is a great<br />

deal of variability in movements. Seemingly identical movements are<br />

not exactly alike. However, these variations occur within a restricted<br />

bandwidth. As a result, one approach to studying motor control is to<br />

examine the variability of movements (Marteniuk & MacKenzie, 1990;<br />

Newel1 & Corcos, 1993; Worringham, 1987,1991).<br />

When our ‘triangular’ modelling strategy was introduced in<br />

<strong>Chapter</strong> 1 (Figure 1.3), it was noted that existing models suggest fur-<br />

ther experiments, the results of which might suggest more compre-<br />

hensive models. Since Arbib’s conceptual model first appeared in the<br />

literature, experimental evidence has shown that size information not<br />

only affects the grasping component but also the transport component.<br />

Recent experiments indicate that visual and mechanical perturbations<br />

affect the kinematics of the transport component as well as the manipu-<br />

lative or grasping component. This suggests a functional coupling of

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!