17.01.2013 Views

Chapter 2. Prehension

Chapter 2. Prehension

Chapter 2. Prehension

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

<strong>Chapter</strong> 5 - Movement Before Contact 173<br />

tween pad and palm opposition as separate prehensile categories, for<br />

planning andor execution. Once pad opposition is chosen, either one<br />

or more fingers can be mapped onto VF2, to oppose the thumb as<br />

VF1. In Sivak (1989) the transport velocity and deceleration phase of<br />

the movement was sensitive only to the distinction between pad and<br />

palm opposition. For cylindrical objects <strong>2.</strong>5 cm in diameter or less,<br />

the number of real anatomical fingers mapped onto VF2 in opposition<br />

to thumb may discriminate the aperture evolution between collective<br />

fingers in palm opposition, collective fingers in pad opposition and in-<br />

dependent fiiger movement in pad opposition.<br />

Recent evidence by Castiello, Bennett and Stelmach (1993),<br />

showed a clear distinction between whether VF2 is mapped into one or<br />

more fingers in opposition to VF1 (as the thumb). Further, they<br />

demonstrated the importance of a natural mapping between object size<br />

and grasp. They compared blocked size and perturbed size conditions<br />

in which subjects grasped and lifted with natural grasps (i.e, VF2<br />

mapped onto the index finger for a 0.7 cm diameter object, compared<br />

to VF2 mapped onto index, middle, ring and little fingers for an 8.0<br />

cm diameter object) or with instructions for one of these grasps (called<br />

precision or whole hand grasps) for both small and large cylinders.<br />

Results indicated that with a natural grasp, there was little or no<br />

increase in movement time for selecting a new grasp type matched to<br />

the object size. In contrast, with instructed grasps, movement<br />

durations increased for adjustments of aperture to a size perturbation.<br />

Like Paulignan, Jeannerod, MacKenzie, and Marteniuk (1991),<br />

adjustments for perturbations from small to large were more difficult<br />

than from large to small. They also found that perturbations requiring<br />

a change from collective fingers to the index finger in opposition to the<br />

thumb (i.e., large to small perturbations) yielded a differentiable<br />

pattern in hand shaping (independent spatial path of the index finger<br />

from the others) about 173 ms after onset of perturbation of object<br />

size. Note that this time is substantially shorter than the 300 - 350 ms<br />

reported by Paulignan et al. for a readjustment to increase aperture<br />

between thumb and index (using pad opposition) with perturbation to<br />

object size.<br />

Further research on the kinematics prior to contact is needed to un-<br />

derstand how opposition space is set up with different types of oppo-<br />

sitions. It is clear that the different opposition types show distinctive<br />

hand configurations (obviously!), as well as corresponding changes in<br />

the kinematics of transport. Following from the above research, more<br />

experiments are needed with a greater range of object properties and<br />

task requirements, as we discussed earlier in our analysis of different

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!