17.01.2013 Views

Chapter 2. Prehension

Chapter 2. Prehension

Chapter 2. Prehension

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

200 THE PHASES OF PREHENSION<br />

might be due to visual feedback correction during the second phase,<br />

the second phase exists in conditions where vision is lacking, arguing<br />

against the notion that the second phase is due to the need for visual<br />

feedback corrections. Importantly, however, when visual information<br />

is available (or possibly available), it will be used. With vision,<br />

movements are longer. Finally, an important issue is the distinction<br />

between the use of peripheral and central vision. According to Sivak<br />

and MacKenzie (1992), when only peripheral vision is available, both<br />

the grasping component and transport components are affected.<br />

Movements are slower and location is more uncertain, when the<br />

temporal link is broken between transport and grasping.<br />

Two major goals seem to be at work, which in their own way in-<br />

fluence both the arm and the hand. Firstly, perception of the location<br />

of the object influences movement parameters; uncertainty in object lo-<br />

cation dictates slowing down in the vicinity of the object, particularly<br />

if the objective is not to bowl it over or crush it. This affects both the<br />

transport component (change in velocity profile) and the grasping<br />

component (open hand before contact is anticipated). Secondly, per-<br />

ception of force-related object properties (e.g., weight, surface tex-<br />

ture, surface sizes) and goals for task performance (e.g., direction and<br />

type of motions to impart, forces to apply) affect the transport compo-<br />

nent (kinematic and kinetic effects effects) and the grasping component<br />

(posturing force generation muscles for the task).<br />

Various models have been proposed for trajectory planning.<br />

Bullock and Grossberg (1989) used an internal model of muscle<br />

length in their VITE model and a time-varying GO signal to update the<br />

model. Kawato et al. (1987) suggested a model for combining three<br />

controllers: a slow cortical feedback controller, a faster cerebellar<br />

feedback controller, and a even faster cerebellar feedforward<br />

controller. Jordan (1988) designed a two tiered network for first<br />

learning the forward kinematics of an arm and then learning sequences<br />

of movements. Constraints were identified to smooth out the<br />

movements and solve ill-posed problems. Massone and Bizzi (1989)<br />

used the Jordan network to generate trajectories of muscle activations.<br />

Instead of derived constraints, they minimized a potential-energy cost<br />

function in order to generate unique solutions.<br />

The CNS may be trying to get some parameters into the ‘right<br />

ballpark’ which can then be fine tuned. Prehensile planning can be<br />

performed by a network of ‘neuron-like’ processes working together<br />

to compute a desired posture and position and orientation from<br />

estimated object location, orientation, and size. These desired values<br />

can then generalized into some ballpark for performing the first phase

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!