17.01.2013 Views

Chapter 2. Prehension

Chapter 2. Prehension

Chapter 2. Prehension

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

184 THE PHASES OF PREHENSION<br />

(1982b), this information appears to selectively contribute to the plan-<br />

ning and control of transport of the hand to the object, not to the for-<br />

mation of the grip. Under natural viewing, central vision provides<br />

important information about object size (and possibly other intrinsic<br />

object characteristics), for accurate grip calibration and hand posturing<br />

in opposition space18. This objective object size information con-<br />

tributes to the organization and control of both the grasp and transport<br />

components. Without the high resolution provided by central vision,<br />

subjects adopt a tactile control strategy, increasing the length of time<br />

they are in contact with the object, in order to successfully complete<br />

the grasp.<br />

Accurate distance information appears to be provided by binocular<br />

vision; with only monocular viewing, subjects appeared to underesti-<br />

mate the distance to objects, as well as their size (Servos, Goodale &<br />

Jakobson, 1992). Combining three oblong object sizes with three<br />

movement distances, they found that when viewing the objects<br />

monocularly (with the preferred eye), movement times were slower,<br />

peak velocities and accelerations were lower than with binocular<br />

vision. As well, greater time was spent in the deceleration phase after<br />

peak velocity with only monocular vision (583 ms or 69%), compared<br />

to binocular vision (390 ms or 63%). These viewing effects interacted<br />

with object distance on kinematic peaks, e.g., the effects of distance<br />

on peak velocity replicated earlier work but were attenuated in the<br />

monocular viewing condition. Subjects always had a smaller peak<br />

aperture under monocular viewing conditions (84 mm) compared to<br />

binocular ones (90 mm). Thus, it appeared that subjects were under-<br />

estimating both the distance and the size of the objects. With both<br />

monocular and binocular viewing, the maximum aperture showed a<br />

similar calibration to visually observed intrinsic properties (i.e., slopes<br />

of the function relating maximum aperture to object size were not<br />

provided, but appear similar, even though with monocular vision,<br />

maximum aperture was consistently smaller). Although not<br />

mentioned, it is assumed that under all conditions, subjects accurately<br />

18 Sivak & MacKenzie (1992) noted with interest neurodevelopmental differences<br />

between central and peripheral regions of the retina. The retina is fully developed at<br />

birth except for the foveal area which is not completely functional until about 6<br />

months of age. This is about the same time at which pad opposition emerges in<br />

infancy (see von Hofsten & Ronnqvist, 1988). Neural, behavioral and functional<br />

parallels in foveal development for foveal grasping and the development of the<br />

somesthetic macula of the finger pads for pad opposition is an inmguing area for<br />

further exploration.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!