17.01.2013 Views

Chapter 2. Prehension

Chapter 2. Prehension

Chapter 2. Prehension

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

146 THE PHASES OF PREHENSION<br />

the object this opening must correspond to the length of the opposition<br />

vector; in addition, the fingerpads must be aligned along the orienta-<br />

tion of the opposition vector. As we will see, the grip aperture may be<br />

a higher order control variable.<br />

Considering the black box introduced in Figure 1.2, we can con-<br />

sider all these kinematic measures of prehensile behavior as outputs.<br />

We saw grasp types and opposition space parameters and outputs in<br />

<strong>Chapter</strong> <strong>2.</strong> Jeannerod introduced the important distinction between<br />

intrinsic and extrinsic object properties accessible through vision; he<br />

also addressed the issue of the role of visual information. In the fol-<br />

lowing sections, we consider what has been learned about object<br />

properties and task requirements, and the roles of sensory informa-<br />

tion, through research analyzing the kinematics of movements, pri-<br />

marily using pad opposition (or precision grip), but also using palm<br />

opposition (or power grasp).<br />

5.4.1 Task requirements and object properties<br />

Further exploring the kinematics of grasping and aiming tasks,<br />

Marteniuk, MacKenzie, Jeannerod, Athenes, and Dugas (1987) varied<br />

precision requirements, showing how intention, context, and object<br />

properties affect timing parameters of prehensile movements. Table<br />

5.1 summarizes the experimental conditions and results. In the first<br />

experiment, they varied the goal, asking subjects to either point at or<br />

grasp medium sized disks. In the second experiment, they varied<br />

object fragility, by asking subjects to either grasp a light bulb or a<br />

tennis ball. In the third experiment, the subjects had to grasp a disk<br />

and either fit it carefully or throw it, thus varying the movement intent.<br />

Fitts’ Law predicts that MT increases with the precision requirements<br />

of aiming or placing, but only addresses the effect of amplitude and<br />

target width on movement time. Marteniuk et al. (1987) bring in task<br />

influences (e.g., context, intent) as well as other object properties<br />

(e.g., fragility). In addition to movement time, Marteniuk et al.<br />

analyzed the velocity profile and separated the acceleration phase<br />

(before peak velocity) from the deceleration phase (after peak<br />

velocity). They observed (see Table 5.1) that the percentage of time in<br />

the deceleration phase was longer for grasping than pointing, for<br />

grasping the light bulb than grasping the tennis ball, and for fitting<br />

rather than throwing. In arguing that all these effects could be due to a<br />

‘task precision’ requirement, they demonstrated that the increased MT<br />

is due to the disproportionately lengthened deceleration phase.<br />

Marteniuk et al. suggested that the duration of the deceleration phase

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!