17.01.2013 Views

Chapter 2. Prehension

Chapter 2. Prehension

Chapter 2. Prehension

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

<strong>Chapter</strong> 5 - Movement Before Contact 185<br />

calibrate the hand opening at contact to successfully grasp the object.<br />

Under both binocular and monocular viewing conditions, the objects<br />

could be seen in foveal vision. Recall that with only central vision,<br />

Sivak also found that subjects underestimated the distance to objects,<br />

but calibrated the grip to the size of the object, in exactly the same way<br />

as with normal, binocular vision (Sivak, 1989; Sivak & MacKenzie,<br />

1992).<br />

Evidence indicates that some sort of proprioceptive information is<br />

also crucial to the preshaping of the hand. For patients having no sen-<br />

sory information from the periphery (from tactile, muscle, joint and<br />

tendon receptors) or having lesions in the parietal cortex, preshaping<br />

of the hand could only be accomplished with vision (Jeannerod,<br />

1986). Jeannerod studied a patient who lost all sensation on her right<br />

side due to the blood flow being blocked to her left anterior parietal<br />

cortex (patient 6, R.S. in Jeannerod, 1986). As seen on the left of<br />

Figure 5.28, she could preshape her normal left hand as she reached<br />

out to grasp the object under visual control. The center column of the<br />

figure shows her affected right hand doing the same task, much<br />

slower, and with more corrections under visually guided control. She<br />

could preshape the hand, but only when the hand was within her eye-<br />

sight. On the right side of the figure, it is shown that she had no<br />

ability to preshape the hand without visual feedback. It appears that<br />

with interruption of the somatosensory pathways, subjects need visual<br />

information in order to configure the hand. Without visual<br />

infomation, the affected hand cannot be preshaped, and the transport<br />

component is also affected.<br />

In addition to lesions in central pathways, disruptions in peripheral<br />

pathways can lead to impaired motor performance. In Rothwell et al.<br />

(1982), a patient known as G.O. lost light touch, vibration and<br />

proprioceptive sensation due to a sensory peripheral neuropathylg. He<br />

was able to perform a variety of tasks, such as thumb positioning and<br />

drawing figures in air with his index finger. Kinematic measures of<br />

his performance in thumb positioning tasks requiring fast and accurate<br />

movements were similar to those of normal subjects. On the slower<br />

movements, he exhibited more variability and less accuracy than<br />

normal subjects, and clearly demonstrated his reliance on vision.<br />

Interestingly, while the subject was able to perform well in laboratory<br />

tests, his need to constantly monitor his movements by vision severely<br />

limited his hand usage in his daily living. Without proprioceptive<br />

information for sustained contractions of muscles, he had lost the<br />

19A disorder in the peripheral nerves.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!