17.01.2013 Views

Chapter 2. Prehension

Chapter 2. Prehension

Chapter 2. Prehension

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

<strong>Chapter</strong> 6 - During Contact 229<br />

assimilation, and control systems analysis. Our point here is that like<br />

the eye, the sensory and motor functions of the hand are intertwined<br />

and indivisible, as the hand is moving to grasp, feeling to grasp,<br />

grasping to feel and grasping to move objects. This ability, called<br />

active touch or haptics, is highly developed in the human sensorimotor<br />

system.<br />

Active touch, or touching was distinguished from passive touch,<br />

or being touched by Gibson (1962), who noted that with active touch,<br />

the impression on the skin is brought about by the perceiver. Active<br />

touch, our concern here, is an active exploratory sense. Through<br />

exploratory movements, tactile scanning can occur, providing<br />

information about object properties.<br />

6.<strong>2.</strong>1 Wielding a stick: feeling object length<br />

In an extensive analysis of perception of length, weight, and<br />

centre of mass in lifted rods, Hoisington (1920) showed that for non-<br />

visual perception of length of a rod lifted or held in palm opposition,<br />

the underlying essential condition is a ratio between two opposing<br />

pressures, held together by directional and temporal relations; in<br />

addition to the two opposed pressures are the absolute intensity of the<br />

impressions, their temporal course, the pressure gradient formed and<br />

the muscle and strain sensations from the hand and arm. Center of<br />

mass of the object bears a constant direct relation to the ratio of<br />

opposing pressures, but is removed from the experienced perception.<br />

Perceptions of length reachable by hand-held rods, partial and<br />

total rod lengths have been evaluated by Burton, Solomon, and<br />

Turvey who suggested that the moments of inertia provided the kinetic<br />

bases for judgements of rod length (Solomon & Turvey, 1988;<br />

Solomon, Turvey & Burton, 1989; Burton & Turvey, 1991). To hold<br />

stationary a rod held in the hand, the forces and torques that must be<br />

applied are a function of the type of opposition (in this case, palm<br />

opposition), virtual finger mapping, hand configuration, hand<br />

placement on the rod, and the physical dimensions of the object.<br />

Placing added weights and the hand at different positions along the<br />

length of the rod altered perceived length in predictable ways<br />

according to the center of mass, and moment arm length, consistent<br />

with the earlier statements of Hoisington (1920) on ratios of opposing<br />

pressures and intensities in the hand and arm. Burton and Turvey<br />

(1991) address the questions of corresponding deformation patterns in<br />

the hand and how prior intents are functionally equivalent to physical<br />

constraints that harness dynamics to form task-specific mechanisms

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!