17.01.2013 Views

Chapter 2. Prehension

Chapter 2. Prehension

Chapter 2. Prehension

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

318 CONSTRAINTS AND PHASES<br />

spent in the deceleration phase was predicted by ID as well or better<br />

than MT. This was not the case for acceleration time. Only movement<br />

amplitude affected the time to peak velocity. Thus, amplitude and<br />

target size effects were dissociable in that the shape of the tangential<br />

velocity profile was a function of target size (accuracy), and the peak<br />

speed along the path of the trajectories was scaled according to<br />

movement amplitude.<br />

In grasping and aiming tasks (Marteniuk et al. 1987), precision<br />

requirements were varied, showing how intention, context, and object<br />

properties affect timing parameters of prehensile movements. The first<br />

experiment varied goal (pointing to a target or grasping a disk); the<br />

second one varied object properties (grasping compliant tennis ball or<br />

fragile light bulb); the third one varied movement intent (placing or<br />

throwing an object. Using Fitts’ Law to equalize the ID’S, it was<br />

observed that the grasping took longer than pointing. The percentage<br />

of time in the deceleration phase was longer for grasping than<br />

pointing, for grasping the light bulb than grasping the tennis ball, and<br />

for placing than throwing. They argued that all these effects could be<br />

due to the requirement for ‘precision’. Fitts’ Law predicts that MT<br />

increases with aiming precision requirements, but they demonstrate<br />

this increase is due to the lengthening of the deceleration phase<br />

disproportionately to the rest of the movement. As well, they show<br />

other influences, such as object properties and task intent. Less<br />

variability between conditions was seen in the early or acceleration<br />

phase of the movement, and more variability during the deceleration<br />

phase. They argued that the early part of the movement is more likely<br />

to be directly influenced by central stereotyped movement planning or<br />

programming, while the later part of the movement, during the<br />

deceleration phase, uses more sensorimotor adjustments for<br />

controlling the movement, causing more variability. Increasing<br />

precision requirements of a task may induce subjects to use more<br />

sensory information, particularly in the ‘homing in’ part of the task.<br />

This relates to Jeannerod (1984), where it was argued that an initial<br />

ballistic phase places the hand into the vicinity of the target, and a<br />

second phase using feedback guides the hand to the target. It is<br />

interesting to note that in pointing tasks, where subjects pointed to<br />

imaginary targets in space, velocity profiles are symmetrical (Atkeson<br />

& Hollerbach, 1985).<br />

Another dimension to task requirements, in contrast to precision,<br />

is the anticipated forces acting in the task. In Iberall et al. (1986), an<br />

informal study is reported to observe the effect of anticipated forces on<br />

the posture chosen to grasp a vertically standing dowel. Subjects were

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!