05.06.2014 Views

A thesis submitted in partial fulfilment of - Queen Margaret University

A thesis submitted in partial fulfilment of - Queen Margaret University

A thesis submitted in partial fulfilment of - Queen Margaret University

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

larger <strong>in</strong>tracranial capacity could withstand more bra<strong>in</strong> age<strong>in</strong>g (or <strong>in</strong>jury) before<br />

cognitive impairment becomes detectible than smaller <strong>in</strong>tracranial capacity<br />

(Kesler et al., 2003). Active processes considered to be factors <strong>in</strong>volved <strong>in</strong><br />

accumulat<strong>in</strong>g cerebral reserve <strong>in</strong>clude the <strong>in</strong>tellectual challenges experienced<br />

throughout our lifetime, such as <strong>in</strong>telligence, education and occupational levels<br />

(Staff et al., 2004; Whalley et al., 2004; Kesler et al., 2003; Plassman, Welsh,<br />

Helms, Brandt, Page, Breitner, 1995).<br />

Some studies have attempted to evaluate the impact <strong>of</strong> age<strong>in</strong>g on cognitive<br />

decl<strong>in</strong>e. One such study <strong>in</strong> Scotland replicated the Scottish Mental Survey <strong>of</strong><br />

1932 (school children aged 11) with 235 <strong>of</strong> the same participants (aged 77 at<br />

follow-up) compar<strong>in</strong>g the f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs to the data <strong>of</strong> 1932 and also provided MRI<br />

data for 98 <strong>of</strong> these participants (Staff et al., 2004). Unlike Kesler et al. (2003)<br />

cerebral size was not supported as a passive factor <strong>in</strong> cognitive reserve. It was<br />

reported that childhood <strong>in</strong>telligence contributed to non-verbal reason<strong>in</strong>g abilities<br />

at age 77 years and both education and a cognitively complex occupation<br />

predicted higher cognitive ability <strong>in</strong> old age. Additionally, relationships were<br />

found between memory and education, memory and occupation, as well as<br />

reason<strong>in</strong>g and occupation. An <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g trend <strong>in</strong> this replicated study <strong>in</strong>dicated<br />

a substantial stability <strong>in</strong> the rank-order<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> human <strong>in</strong>telligence across the life<br />

span where children who scored highly <strong>in</strong> assessments at age 11 tended to<br />

score highly at age 77 and vice versa. A cross-sequential study <strong>in</strong> Seattle also<br />

recorded the <strong>in</strong>tellectual and cognitive capacity <strong>of</strong> a population from childhood to<br />

adulthood (over 5000 participants) (Schaie, 1994). This study reported a decl<strong>in</strong>e<br />

<strong>of</strong> <strong>in</strong>tellect with age<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> areas <strong>of</strong> <strong>in</strong>ductive reason<strong>in</strong>g, spatial orientation,<br />

perceptual speed and verbal memory, however, there was much less age<br />

related decl<strong>in</strong>e <strong>in</strong> verbal and numerical ability.<br />

It is however difficult to establish basel<strong>in</strong>e measures <strong>of</strong> active factors such as<br />

pre-morbid <strong>in</strong>telligence and cognitive status, to enable comparison with a<br />

36

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!