14.01.2013 Views

Interrogations-and-Confessions-Handbook

Interrogations-and-Confessions-Handbook

Interrogations-and-Confessions-Handbook

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Why do Suspects Confess? Empirical Findings 133<br />

apprehending accomplices <strong>and</strong> clearing up other crimes. Having observed the<br />

interrogation of 127 suspects <strong>and</strong> interviewed the detectives involved, the authors<br />

found that interrogation was only necessary for solving the crime in about<br />

17% of cases (Wald et al., 1967; Table F-6, p. 1585). They concluded:<br />

Thus, even in a force as scientifically advanced as Los Angeles’, there is strong<br />

evidence that confessions are of small importance, since arrests can be made only<br />

where the crime is for the most part already solved because such substantial<br />

evidence is available before interrogation (p. 1588).<br />

In a more recent study, Leo (1996a) found that out of 182 cases observed, the<br />

strength of the evidence against the suspect prior to interrogation was weak in<br />

33% of cases <strong>and</strong> is unlikely to have led to a charge without a confession. Leo<br />

concluded:<br />

Suspects who provide incriminating information to detectives are significantly more<br />

likely to be treated differently at every subsequent stage of the criminal process than<br />

those suspects who do not provide incriminating information during interrogation<br />

(p. 298, original author’s italics).<br />

Leo found that suspects who incriminated themselves during interrogation<br />

were 20% more likely to be charged by prosecutors, 24% less likely to have<br />

their case dismissed, 25% more likely to have their cases resolved by plea bargaining<br />

<strong>and</strong> 26% more likely to be found guilty <strong>and</strong> convicted. In addition, those<br />

who had confessed received heavier sentences following conviction.<br />

On the basis of the review the American literature, Cassell (1996a) estimates<br />

that confessions are necessary for a conviction in about 24% of cases.<br />

HOW COMMONLY DO SUSPECTS CONFESS?<br />

Research shows that many suspects interrogated at police stations confess to<br />

the crime of which they are accused. A further proportion of suspects make<br />

self-incriminating admissions that fall short of a full confession. In ‘run-ofthe-mill’<br />

criminal cases in Engl<strong>and</strong> a confession or an admission typically<br />

occurs at the beginning of an interview <strong>and</strong> the suspect typically sticks to his<br />

chosen position throughout the interview irrespective of the technique used<br />

(Baldwin, 1993; Evans, 1993; Irving & McKenzie, 1989; Moston, Stephenson &<br />

Williamson, 1992; Pearse <strong>and</strong> Gudjonsson, 1996a; Pearse et al., 1998). This<br />

strongly indicates that once the suspect enters the interview he has already<br />

decided whether or not to make an admission or a full confession. Inbau et al.<br />

(2001) claim the contrary in the USA. Their argument is that the great majority<br />

of suspects initially deny their involvement in the offence, <strong>and</strong> with the assistance<br />

of the Reid Technique of interrogation about 80% of the denials change<br />

to a confession (see Chapter 1).<br />

There is no empirical evidence to support these most extraordinary claims.<br />

The statement is presumably used to impress upon the readers the high success<br />

rate of the Reid Technique.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!