14.01.2013 Views

Interrogations-and-Confessions-Handbook

Interrogations-and-Confessions-Handbook

Interrogations-and-Confessions-Handbook

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Suggestibility: Empirical Findings 405<br />

The resisters were found to be significantly more intelligent <strong>and</strong> less suggestible<br />

than the alleged false confessors. A particularly significant finding was the difference<br />

between the two groups in the type of suggestibility that related to the<br />

ability to resist interrogative pressure. These were the Shift <strong>and</strong> Yield 2 scores<br />

on the GSS 1.<br />

I identified two limitations with the study. First, the number of subjects in<br />

each group was very small. Secondly, part of the difference in suggestibility<br />

between the two groups could have been influenced by the differences in IQ<br />

between the two groups. Sharrock (1988) goes further <strong>and</strong> states that the difference<br />

in the IQ between the two groups ‘accounts for most of the difference<br />

in their suggestibility’ (p. 220). Sharrock’s bold statement seems to have been<br />

based on the erroneous assumption that, since there is a certain negative relationship<br />

between IQ <strong>and</strong> suggestibility, this is likely to have mediated the<br />

differences in suggestibility between the alleged false confessors <strong>and</strong> resisters.<br />

I take the view that suggestibility is mediated by a number of factors, intelligence<br />

being only one of them. However, Sharrock raised an important point,<br />

which warranted a study where the IQ of the two groups are controlled for.<br />

In an attempt to investigate Sharrock’s observation, I extended <strong>and</strong> replicated<br />

the 1984 study (Gudjonsson, 1991c). The resisters <strong>and</strong> alleged false confessors<br />

were different to those used in the 1984 study <strong>and</strong> the number of subjects<br />

in each group was much larger than in the previous study. Perhaps most importantly,<br />

however, the two groups of subjects were matched with respect to age,<br />

sex, intelligence <strong>and</strong> memory capacity. The study also had the advantage over<br />

the 1984 study in that the subjects had all completed both the GSS 1 <strong>and</strong> the<br />

GCS. Thus, both suggestibility <strong>and</strong> compliance were measured. It was hypothesized<br />

that, even with intelligence <strong>and</strong> memory capacity controlled for, the two<br />

groups would still show significant differences with regard to suggestibility <strong>and</strong><br />

compliance, the main difference being related to the ability of subjects to resist<br />

interrogative pressure, as measured by the GCS <strong>and</strong> the Yield 2 <strong>and</strong> Shift parts<br />

of the GSS 1.<br />

The most important finding was that highly significant differences emerged<br />

between alleged false confessors <strong>and</strong> resisters after their intelligence <strong>and</strong> memory<br />

capacity had been controlled for. This has important implications for the<br />

assessment of retracted confession cases. First, it demonstrates that the assessment<br />

of suggestibility <strong>and</strong> compliance, which are theoretically construed as<br />

overlapping characteristics (Gudjonsson, 1989c), contributes to discriminating<br />

between the two groups largely independently of the subjects’ level of intelligence.<br />

In other words, intelligence may be an important factor in differentiating<br />

between alleged false confessors <strong>and</strong> retractors, but other factors, such<br />

as suggestibility <strong>and</strong> compliance, are also important <strong>and</strong> should not be underestimated.<br />

Secondly, the present findings are a clear warning to clinicians<br />

carrying out a forensic assessment. That is, even though suggestibility <strong>and</strong> intelligence<br />

are modestly correlated, it is erroneous to assume that differences in<br />

suggestibility <strong>and</strong> compliance are largely or necessarily mediated by differences<br />

in intelligence, as Sharrock (1988) postulated.<br />

The mean suggestibility scores in this recent study are very similar to those<br />

found in the 1984 study for alleged false confessors <strong>and</strong> resisters. Furthermore,

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!