14.01.2013 Views

Interrogations-and-Confessions-Handbook

Interrogations-and-Confessions-Handbook

Interrogations-and-Confessions-Handbook

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

They state:<br />

The Psychology of False Confession: Research <strong>and</strong> Theoretical Issues 183<br />

Many confessed criminal offenders will subsequently deny their guilt <strong>and</strong> allege<br />

that they either did not confess or else were forced or induced to do so by physical<br />

abuse, threats, or promise of leniency. Occasionally, the defendant in a criminal<br />

case will even go so far as to say that he was compelled to sign a written confession<br />

without reading it or having had it read to him, or that he was forced to place his<br />

signature on a blank sheet of paper <strong>and</strong> all that appears above it was inserted<br />

later (p. 375).<br />

In the third edition of the book, Inbau, Reid <strong>and</strong> Buckley (1986) used the words<br />

‘Most confessed criminal offenders . . . ’ (p. 176). In the fourth edition the word<br />

‘most’ has been replaced with the word ‘many’. The reason for this change<br />

is not explained. The most likely explanation is that Inbau, Reid <strong>and</strong> Buckley<br />

(1986) overstated or exaggerated the position <strong>and</strong> this was corrected in the later<br />

edition. It is important to note that no empirical evidence is provided in either<br />

edition of the book for the claims made about retractions; it is undoubtedly<br />

based on the authors’ impressions rather than empirical data. Support for this<br />

comes from the following statement:<br />

In our experience, the vast majority of retracted confessions are, in fact, trustworthy<br />

statements coming from the person who committed the crime (p. 437).<br />

Inbau, Reid <strong>and</strong> Buckley (1986) do not consider the possibility that anybody who<br />

retracts a previously made confession could possibly be innocent. They work on<br />

the misguided assumption that their recommended tactics <strong>and</strong> techniques never<br />

induce an innocent person to falsely confess. There are sufficient numbers of<br />

proven cases of innocent persons retracting false confessions to demonstrate<br />

that this belief of Inbau, Reid <strong>and</strong> Buckley is unfounded. What their statement<br />

does highlight is the fact that criminal suspects very commonly retract their<br />

previously made confession to the police <strong>and</strong> give various excuses for having<br />

done so. Inbau, Reid <strong>and</strong> Buckley are wrong to assume that all such retractions<br />

involve guilty people claiming to be innocent.<br />

In the more recent edition of their book, Inbau et al. (2001) do accept that<br />

false confessions occur:<br />

There is no question that interrogations have resulted in false confessions from<br />

innocent suspects (p. 411).<br />

However, when citing a case involving a false confession, Inbau et al. do not<br />

cite a case involving police coercion. Instead, they cite a case of a 16-year-old<br />

youth who had been persuaded by his father to confess to a murder the father<br />

had committed. This suggests that Inbau et al. are still reluctant to accept<br />

that false confession may occur as a result of interrogation relying on the Reid<br />

Technique.<br />

The process of denial may continue to operate after people have been convicted.<br />

Kennedy <strong>and</strong> Grubin (1992) found that convicted sex offenders who<br />

pleaded guilty to their offence at the time of their trial begin to deny their offence<br />

once they are in prison. This process of denial may serve some important

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!