14.01.2013 Views

Interrogations-and-Confessions-Handbook

Interrogations-and-Confessions-Handbook

Interrogations-and-Confessions-Handbook

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

368 A Psychology of <strong>Interrogations</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Confessions</strong><br />

they are less imaginative <strong>and</strong> creative, which impairs their normal tendency to<br />

confabulate.<br />

SUGGESTIBILITY AND HYPNOTIC SUSCEPTIBILITY<br />

I argued in Chapter 13 that the susceptibility to hypnosis is related to primary<br />

suggestibility, using Eysenck’s conventional classification, whereas interrogative<br />

suggestibility is a special type of suggestibility that is unrelated to suggestibility<br />

of the primary type <strong>and</strong> only relates to the more elusive category of<br />

secondary suggestibility.<br />

Evidence that interrogative suggestibility differs from susceptibility to hypnosis<br />

comes from six empirical studies. In the first study, Hardarson (1985)<br />

found no significant correlation ( r = 0.15) between scores on the Harvard Group<br />

Scale of Hypnotic Susceptibility <strong>and</strong> interrogative suggestibility, as measured<br />

by the GSS 1, among 40 Icel<strong>and</strong>ic University students.<br />

In two different experiments, one comprising university students <strong>and</strong> one<br />

psychiatric patients, Young, Bentall, Slade <strong>and</strong> Dewey (1987) correlated the<br />

GSS 1 Total Suggestibility score with the Barber Suggestibility Scale (Barber &<br />

Calverley, 1964). The Barber Suggestibility Scale consists of eight test suggestions<br />

that are theoretically related to primary suggestibility. None of the three<br />

scores on the Barber Suggestibility Scale, which comprised the subjects’ responses<br />

to suggestions, their rated subjective involvement in the tasks, <strong>and</strong><br />

their verbalized resistance to the suggestions, correlated with interrogative<br />

suggestibility.<br />

Register <strong>and</strong> Kihlstrom (1988) used a variant of the GSS 1 during an experiment<br />

into hypnosis. The subjects were 40 college students, who had all<br />

completed the Harvard Group Scale of Hypnotic Susceptibility <strong>and</strong> were interrogated<br />

after hypnotic induction. Negative feedback was not administered;<br />

instead the interrogation questions were repeated without any explicit negative<br />

feedback. No significant difference in interrogative suggestibility was found between<br />

hypnotizable <strong>and</strong> non-hypnotizable subjects. The authors concluded that<br />

the results<br />

. . . support Gudjonsson’s (1987) hypothesis that interrogative suggestibility is<br />

independent of suggestibility as measured in a hypnotic context (p. 556).<br />

Gwynn <strong>and</strong> Spanos (1996) cite two studies where interrogative suggestibility,<br />

as measured by the GSS, was not found to be correlated with hypnotizability.<br />

In both studies hypnotizability was measured by the Carleton University<br />

Responsiveness to Suggestion Scale (CURSS). In the first study (Gwynn,<br />

Spanos, Nancoo & Chow, 1995, unpublished manuscript), 120 subjects who<br />

had previously completed the CURSS were administered the GSS. The CURSS<br />

score did not correlate significantly with Yield, Shift or Total Suggestibility. The<br />

second study (Gordon, Gwynn & Spanos, 1993) also involved 120 subjects, who<br />

had previously completed the CURSS. One half of the subjects were later tested<br />

on the GSS. No significant correlation was found between the hypnotizability<br />

score <strong>and</strong> the GSS scores.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!