14.01.2013 Views

Interrogations-and-Confessions-Handbook

Interrogations-and-Confessions-Handbook

Interrogations-and-Confessions-Handbook

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

200 A Psychology of <strong>Interrogations</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Confessions</strong><br />

interrogator argues for the existence of a mental disorder that would explain<br />

the lack of memory for the crime (e.g. multiple personality, dissociation<br />

etc). These ploys tend to undermine the confidence that suspects have in<br />

their ability to accurately recall that they had not committed the alleged<br />

crime.<br />

6. The interrogator dem<strong>and</strong>s that the suspect accepts his premises <strong>and</strong> explanations<br />

of the alleged crime.<br />

7. The interrogator attempts to induce fear in the suspect’s mind about the potential<br />

consequences of repeated denials. Ofshe maintains that the tactics<br />

of interrogation in which American interrogators are trained will render<br />

them able to produce the result without being aware that they are generating<br />

a false confession (Ofshe, 1989).<br />

Not all of the above tactics <strong>and</strong> techniques are likely to be evident in every<br />

case of coerced–internalized false confession, but they are the types of factor<br />

that facilitate the process. What seems to happen is that these tactics <strong>and</strong><br />

techniques make the suspect lose confidence in his memory, he or she becomes<br />

very confused about what is happening <strong>and</strong> as a result is unable critically <strong>and</strong><br />

rationally to evaluate the predicament he or she is in.<br />

The four individuals that comprise Ofshe’s small study of coerced–<br />

internalized false confessors had all been assessed psychologically. None of<br />

them were considered mentally ill. Three personality factors were evident although<br />

not particularly extreme.<br />

1. Good trust of people in authority.<br />

2. Lack of self-confidence.<br />

3. Heightened suggestibility.<br />

Ofshe argues that these personality characteristics made the false confessors<br />

vulnerable to the influence of manipulative forms of interrogation. In his view,<br />

these vulnerabilities ‘probably contributed most significantly to the speed with<br />

which the process of thought reform could be carried out’ (p. 14). The implication<br />

is that suspects who are not in any significant way psychologically vulnerable at<br />

the beginning of the interrogation can make coerced–internalized confessions,<br />

provided they are interrogated for extensive periods of time <strong>and</strong> the ‘relevant’<br />

techniques <strong>and</strong> tactics are used by interrogators.<br />

My own experience of coerced–internalized false confessions supports Ofshe’s<br />

findings, but I would add the following.<br />

� It is specifically the lack of confidence in one’s memory that is important.<br />

Even people with good memories may lack confidence in their memory, or<br />

the confidence that they have in their memory may be easily challenged<br />

<strong>and</strong> undermined by the interrogator.<br />

� As far as suggestibility is concerned, it is having problems with discrepancy<br />

detection that is more important than suggestibility per se. It is the failure of<br />

people to differentiate between what is real memory <strong>and</strong> what is suggested<br />

to them that is important. In other words, they are unable to recognize<br />

when errors <strong>and</strong> misinformation are being introduced into their memory<br />

<strong>and</strong> belief system.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!