14.01.2013 Views

Interrogations-and-Confessions-Handbook

Interrogations-and-Confessions-Handbook

Interrogations-and-Confessions-Handbook

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Why do Suspects Confess? Empirical Findings 135<br />

denial in his calculation of the outcome of the interrogation. Forty-one (23%)<br />

suspects fell into this group. The distinction here is between incriminating denials<br />

<strong>and</strong> incriminating admissions. Both can be used against the suspect at<br />

trial.<br />

Second, another source of difference relates to the basis on which the suspects’<br />

confessions are classified. The best methodology is to make the judgement<br />

on the basis of typed transcripts that have been carefully checked for accuracy<br />

against audio-taped interviews (Pearse & Gudjonsson, 1999). In Engl<strong>and</strong> tapes<br />

are not routinely transcribed <strong>and</strong> often only a summary is produced by the police<br />

for judicial purposes. What researchers have tended to do when tapes are available<br />

is to listen to the tapes <strong>and</strong> make a judgement on that basis (Baldwin, 1993;<br />

Moston, Stephenson & Williamson, 1992; Pearse & Gudjonsson, 1996a). Other<br />

researchers have been present during interviews <strong>and</strong> noted evidence of admissions<br />

<strong>and</strong> confessions (Irving, 1980; Irving & McKenzie, 1989; Leo, 1996a). The<br />

weakest methodology is to rely exclusively on the police records, because admissions,<br />

confessions, <strong>and</strong> denials are not always accurately recorded in the<br />

files (Evans, 1993).<br />

Third, the rate of confession is higher in cases that reach court than those<br />

that do not (Cassell, 1996a; Pearse et al., 1998; Phillips & Brown, 1998). Therefore,<br />

focusing only on cases where there is a guilty plea <strong>and</strong> those that reach<br />

court (see e.g. Baldwin & McConville, 1980; Bryan, 1997; Mitchell, 1983; Z<strong>and</strong>er,<br />

1979) undoubtedly gives a misleading <strong>and</strong> exaggerated picture of the overall<br />

confession or admission rates. This has to be taken into consideration when<br />

comparing confession rates across different studies.<br />

Fourth, there are a number of other factors that can also make a difference<br />

to the confession figures, such as adding to the figures suspects who are not<br />

interviewed, which for obvious reasons deflates the confession rate figure. For<br />

example, Cassell <strong>and</strong> Hayman (1998) included in their study 46 suspects who<br />

were not interviewed by the police. This lowered the confession rate from 42<br />

to 33%. The majority (61%) were not interviewed because they could not be<br />

found for interrogation <strong>and</strong> in further 27% of cases there was already an overwhelming<br />

case against the suspect. We do not know the likely confession rate<br />

for the former group but the latter group would probably have had a high confession<br />

rate due to the strength of the existing evidence against them. The only<br />

legitimate cases to include in the figures are those where suspects could not<br />

be interviewed because they invoked their Mir<strong>and</strong>a rights at the beginning of<br />

the interrogation, or in English cases, where suspects exercise their right to<br />

silence.<br />

There are important legal <strong>and</strong> cultural differences between America <strong>and</strong><br />

Engl<strong>and</strong> with regard to suspects invoking their legal rights. For the American<br />

police there are potentially serious consequences when suspects invoke their<br />

Mir<strong>and</strong>a rights, <strong>and</strong> between 4 <strong>and</strong> 78% do so, depending on the study (Leo,<br />

1998). These are potentially more serious than English suspects requesting<br />

legal advice <strong>and</strong> exercising their right to silence. When suspects invoke their<br />

Mir<strong>and</strong>a rights they effectively decline to give the police permission to interview<br />

them <strong>and</strong> they are rarely interviewed subsequently (Cassell & Hayman, 1998;

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!