14.01.2013 Views

Interrogations-and-Confessions-Handbook

Interrogations-and-Confessions-Handbook

Interrogations-and-Confessions-Handbook

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

180 A Psychology of <strong>Interrogations</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Confessions</strong><br />

A confession that contains an apparently detailed special knowledge may<br />

prove to be false (see, for example, Chapter 20). Special knowledge about a<br />

crime can be obtained <strong>and</strong> contaminated through a variety of sources, including<br />

the following.<br />

1. The media.<br />

2. The police.<br />

3. Crime scene visits.<br />

4. Crime scene material, such as photographs.<br />

5. A third party (i.e. being told about it by the real perpetrator).<br />

Any of the above modes of communication can contaminate the original knowledge<br />

the person had about the crime, making it appear as if he is providing<br />

knowledge that only the real perpetrator should possess. The best special<br />

knowledge is that which is only known to the real perpetrator (e.g. where an<br />

undiscovered body or murder weapon has been hidden). Once the police are<br />

in possession of that special knowledge, the possibility of it being communicated<br />

to the suspect during custodial confinement <strong>and</strong> interrogation may be<br />

difficult to rule out. Audio or video recording of all interrogations can be important<br />

to rule out such contamination, although even this procedure may not be<br />

foolproof.<br />

As will be shown in Chapter 18, even if the suspect reveals no special<br />

knowledge, <strong>and</strong> appears to be unfamiliar with the crime, he may still be<br />

convicted on the basis of that confession alone (see the case of Peter Fell in<br />

Chapter 18).<br />

The Discovery of a False Confession<br />

When a false confession occurs, it may be discovered by different means, including<br />

one or more of the following.<br />

1. Discovery that no crime was committed (e.g. an alleged murder victim turns<br />

up alive—see for example, Leo & Ofshe, 1998a; The Earl of Birkenhead,<br />

1938).<br />

2. New forensic evidence, including improved DNA testing capabilities<br />

(Gudjonsson & MacKeith, 1994; Scheck, Neufeld & Dwyer, 2000;<br />

Wambaugh, 1989).<br />

3. New alibi evidence (see Chapter 17 with reference to Gerry Conlan).<br />

4. Newly discovered medical evidence, which would have made it impossible<br />

for the person to have committed the crime (see e.g. Rose, Panter &<br />

Wilkinson, 1998). There may also be evidence that proves that the defendant<br />

could not have committed the crime, because he was in police custody<br />

or prison at the time of the offence (Leo & Ofshe, 1998a).<br />

5. Somebody else confesses <strong>and</strong> is convicted of the offence (Cassell, 1999;<br />

Gudjonsson & MacKeith, 1990; Leo & Ofshe, 1998a).<br />

6. Psychological <strong>and</strong> psychiatric evidence that casts serious doubts on the<br />

veracity of the confession (Gudjonsson, 1995d; Gudjonsson, Kopelman &<br />

MacKeith; 1999; Gudjonsson & Lebegue, 1989;).

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!