14.01.2013 Views

Interrogations-and-Confessions-Handbook

Interrogations-and-Confessions-Handbook

Interrogations-and-Confessions-Handbook

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

386 A Psychology of <strong>Interrogations</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Confessions</strong><br />

although one study, conducted among police detainees, found the reverse pattern<br />

(Gudjonsson, Rutter & Clare, 1995—this unexpected finding is discussed<br />

in detail in Chapter 3). Smith <strong>and</strong> Gudjonsson (1995a), studying forensic inpatients,<br />

found no significant relationship between any of the GSS 2 suggestibility<br />

scores <strong>and</strong> state anxiety at the time of the interrogation, but did so with regard<br />

to Yield 2, Shift <strong>and</strong> Total Suggestibility at one week follow-up. In the studies<br />

measuring ‘state’ <strong>and</strong> ‘trait’ anxiety the Spielberger State–Trait Anxiety<br />

Inventory (STAI, Spielberger, 1983) has been used.<br />

The empirical evidence indicates that there is a poor relationship between<br />

suggestibility <strong>and</strong> trait anxiety as measured by self-report questionnaires. For<br />

example, Haraldsson (1985) found no significant correlation between GSS 1<br />

suggestibility scores <strong>and</strong> neuroticism, which was measured by the Icel<strong>and</strong>ic<br />

version of the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire (EPQ; Eysenck & Haraldsson,<br />

1983). The sample consisted of 54 Icel<strong>and</strong>ic University students.<br />

In the first study on the GSS 1 (Gudjonsson, 1983), I found a low but<br />

significant correlation (r = 0.28, p < 0.05) between total suggestibility <strong>and</strong><br />

Neuroticism as measured by the English version of the EPQ (Eysenck &<br />

Eysenck, 1975).<br />

There is some evidence that suggestibility is more strongly associated with<br />

‘state’ anxiety than ‘trait’ anxiety. The former is typically construed as a transitory<br />

emotional state that is characterized by subjective feelings of apprehension<br />

<strong>and</strong> heightened autonomic nervous system reactivity. Trait anxiety, on the other<br />

h<strong>and</strong>, refers to relatively stable individual differences in anxiety proneness.<br />

In one study (Gudjonsson, 1988a), I set out to investigate the hypothesis that<br />

state anxiety is more strongly associated with suggestibility than is trait anxiety.<br />

I administered the Spielberger State Anxiety Inventory (STAI; Spielberger,<br />

1983) twice to the subjects in the study. The subjects first completed the STAI<br />

prior to the GSS 1 interrogation <strong>and</strong> then after they had been interrogated<br />

<strong>and</strong> given the st<strong>and</strong>ard negative feedback. In contrast to studies utilizing trait<br />

anxiety, some highly significant correlations emerged. The correlations were<br />

consistently higher with the second administration of the STAI than the first.<br />

In addition, Shift <strong>and</strong> Yield 2 correlated significantly more highly with state<br />

anxiety, during both administrations of the STAI, than with Yield 1. The correlations<br />

with Shift were 0.42 <strong>and</strong> 0.69 for the two STAI tests respectively.<br />

The findings from this study support the hypothesis that suggestibility is<br />

strongly associated with state anxiety. This indicates that it is how apprehensive<br />

subjects feel at the time of the interrogation that is more important than<br />

their more generalized anxiety proneness. In addition, state anxiety is clearly<br />

most strongly associated with how subjects react to interrogative pressure<br />

rather than to leading questions per se. This supports my theory (Gudjonsson,<br />

1984a) that Yield 2 <strong>and</strong> Shift scores on the GSS are more linked to anxiety <strong>and</strong><br />

coping processes than Yield 1. The findings complement those of Tata (1983),<br />

who found that negative feedback on the GSS 1 is accompanied by increased<br />

electrodermal reactivity as well as changes in mood as measured by the Multiple<br />

Affect Adjective Checklist (Zuckerman & Lubin, 1965).<br />

In an early study (Gudjonsson & Singh, 1984a), we attempted to validate<br />

the GSS 1 by administering the scale to 31 delinquent <strong>and</strong> adolescent boys

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!