14.01.2013 Views

Interrogations-and-Confessions-Handbook

Interrogations-and-Confessions-Handbook

Interrogations-and-Confessions-Handbook

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

322 A Psychology of <strong>Interrogations</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Confessions</strong><br />

in miscarriages of justice. Clinical impressions by mental health professionals<br />

about the defendant’s intellectual strengths <strong>and</strong> weaknesses are often misleading.<br />

In one case, a prison medical officer described a defendant in court as being<br />

of ‘average intelligence’. It was only during the cross-examination of the doctor,<br />

at my instigation, that it transpired that his conclusion was based on a clinical<br />

impression from a very brief (5–10 minute) interview <strong>and</strong> certainly not on<br />

actual testing. I had tested the defendant <strong>and</strong> found him to have a Full Scale IQ<br />

of 69! This was the first case where I testified in a criminal trial in relation to a<br />

retracted confession. It was at the beginning of 1982. In addition to a significant<br />

intellectual impairment, I had found the defendant to have severe anxiety problems.<br />

The judge admitted the evidence in relation to the IQ, but refused to hear<br />

the evidence in relation to the defendant’s anxiety problems. The judge then<br />

ruled the confession admissible <strong>and</strong> the defendant was convicted of murder.<br />

There is evidence (Clare & Gudjonsson, 1991) that the majority of persons<br />

with a mild learning disability do not realize that they should inform the police<br />

of their disability if detained at a police station. Furthermore, 30% of the participants<br />

in the study stated that they would not tell the police about their learning<br />

disability, because they regarded the information as private <strong>and</strong> personal. This<br />

means that many persons with learning disability may deliberately try to cover<br />

up their deficits, including reading disability, which may inadvertently mislead<br />

the police.<br />

Another problem with the identification of learning disability is that professional<br />

views on what should be the upper limit with regard to IQ vary immensely.<br />

Denkowski <strong>and</strong> Denkowski (1985) <strong>and</strong> Richard, Spencer <strong>and</strong> Spooner<br />

(1980) report that the cut-off scores for legal purposes range from 60 to 78. In<br />

the WAIS-R manual a score below 70 is considered to define learning disability<br />

(the same criterion applies to the WAIS-III). The cut-off point of 70 is quite<br />

arbitrary <strong>and</strong> was chosen on an historical rather than scientific basis. The convention<br />

seems to be not to unnecessarily label people, but keeping to such a low<br />

threshold runs the risk of intellectually impaired defendants not being properly<br />

provided with the assistance they need when interviewed by the police. There<br />

is now a trend not to rely rigidly on fixed IQ scores, but to judge each case on<br />

its own merit (British Psychological Society, 2001). In fact, an IQ of 75, which<br />

represents the bottom five per cent of the adult population, would be more in<br />

line with scientific thinking about a significant impairment <strong>and</strong> abnormality.<br />

An IQ of 70 represents the bottom two per cent of the general population <strong>and</strong><br />

it is the figure that the Lord Chief Justice adopted in the case of Masih ([1986],<br />

Crim.L.R. 395). He considered an IQ of 69 or below as a definition of learning<br />

disability. The problems with this rigid legal definition are that no account<br />

is taken of social functioning <strong>and</strong> the ‘st<strong>and</strong>ard error of measurement’ of any<br />

given intelligence test. In certain cases, an assessment of social functioning is<br />

undoubtedly relevant to the deliberation of the judge <strong>and</strong> jury.<br />

The ‘st<strong>and</strong>ard error of measurement’ of a given test has an important legal<br />

significance (Matarazzo, 1990), because it indicates the actual b<strong>and</strong> of error<br />

around the obtained IQ score. This b<strong>and</strong> of error is associated with variations<br />

in scores when people are tested on different occasions due to inherent<br />

unreliability of the test itself. Therefore, a score obtained from a single test

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!