14.01.2013 Views

Interrogations-and-Confessions-Handbook

Interrogations-and-Confessions-Handbook

Interrogations-and-Confessions-Handbook

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Suggestibility: Historical <strong>and</strong> Theoretical Aspects 341<br />

progressive weights <strong>and</strong> lines, as well as his ‘prestige’ <strong>and</strong> ‘interrogatory’ tests,<br />

were assumed by him to include:<br />

1. obedience to mental influence from another person,<br />

2. the tendency to imitate,<br />

3. influence of a preconceived idea that paralysed the individual’s critical<br />

sense <strong>and</strong><br />

4. expectative attention.<br />

Stukat (1958) found some support for Binet’s theoretical formulation from his<br />

factorial studies, where the first two categories (1 <strong>and</strong> 2 above) were quite<br />

similar to Stukat’s ‘need for conformity’ factors <strong>and</strong> the last two (3 <strong>and</strong> 4) corresponded<br />

to an ‘expectative’ factor.<br />

McDougall (1908), whose definition of suggestion was given earlier, associates<br />

suggestibility with four distinct conditions:<br />

1. abnormal states of the brain (e.g. as during hypnosis, sleep <strong>and</strong> fatigue);<br />

2. deficiency <strong>and</strong> poor organization of knowledge regarding the subject matter<br />

being communicated;<br />

3. the impressive character of the person communicating the suggestion (i.e.<br />

‘prestige’ suggestion) <strong>and</strong><br />

4. the character <strong>and</strong> disposition of the subject.<br />

McDougall thought of the relative strengths of ‘instincts’, ‘assertion’ <strong>and</strong><br />

‘subjection’ as the most crucial conditions determining the individual’s level<br />

of suggestibility. For example, an individual with a strong impulse of selfassertion<br />

when communicating with others of lower status makes the former<br />

non-suggestible to the influence of the latter. McDougall also emphasized the<br />

importance of the person’s knowledge, <strong>and</strong> confidence in his knowledge, as mediating<br />

variables in the susceptibility to suggestion. McDougall’s emphasis on<br />

both motivation <strong>and</strong> cognitive factors in determining suggestibility is fundamental<br />

to the underst<strong>and</strong>ing of secondary suggestibility, including interrogative<br />

suggestibility.<br />

Another theoretical model of relevance to secondary suggestibility is that of<br />

Sherif (1936). He argues that a stimulus is never reacted to in isolation. It is<br />

always experienced, perceived, judged <strong>and</strong> reacted to in relation to other stimuli,<br />

present or past, to which it is fundamentally related. Sherif used the term<br />

‘frame of reference’ to denote these functionally related factors that influence<br />

perceptions <strong>and</strong> judgements,<br />

Coffin (1941) has exp<strong>and</strong>ed Sherif’s theory. He regards suggestion as a<br />

framework response, determined by internal factors (e.g. attitude) <strong>and</strong> external<br />

features of the stimulus situation. When a situation is ‘well structured’<br />

in terms of either attitudinal or situational factors, only those suggestions<br />

which accord with the existing frame of reference are likely to be accepted.<br />

The advantage of the cognitive model of Sherif <strong>and</strong> Coffin is that it is conceptually<br />

simple <strong>and</strong> seems to explain many experimental findings. A possible<br />

weakness is the strong emphasis on the cognitive aspects of internal<br />

factors, because even though suggestions may well function as a frame of reference<br />

usually there are emotional <strong>and</strong> motivational factors involved in the

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!