22.04.2014 Views

Abstracts (PDF file, 1.8MB) - Society for Risk Analysis

Abstracts (PDF file, 1.8MB) - Society for Risk Analysis

Abstracts (PDF file, 1.8MB) - Society for Risk Analysis

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

SRA 2013 Annual Meeting <strong>Abstracts</strong><br />

P.130 Kowalek, Denna; Howard University ;<br />

denna.kowalek@gmail.com<br />

Understanding risk: Applying the CAUSE model in a<br />

content analysis of emergency management organizations<br />

coverage of hurricane Sandy.<br />

Andersen and Spitzberg (2010) state that by many measures,<br />

the world is becoming a more dangerous place. The fact that<br />

there are more people in more places, from more cultures,<br />

often in greater levels of density, means that when disasters<br />

occur, they have the potential to affect more people, and more<br />

organizations and institutions are responsible <strong>for</strong> managing<br />

such disasters. Examining emergency management<br />

organizations communicative messages during Hurricane<br />

Sandy in the fall of 2012 allows risk and crisis communicators<br />

to determine how the hurricane in<strong>for</strong>mation was disseminated,<br />

thus providing further precautionary and preparedness actions.<br />

Understanding how people view disasters and precautionary<br />

and preparedness will help generate more effective risk<br />

communication campaigns. This research will utilize Rowan’s et<br />

al. (2009) CAUSE model as a framework to understand how The<br />

National Weather Service (NOAA), American Red Cross and<br />

FEMA incorporated precautionary and preparedness action into<br />

their coverage of Hurricane Sandy. This will be done to<br />

determine how emergency management organizations created<br />

understanding through their messages. This in<strong>for</strong>mation is<br />

crucial to understand in order to comprehend precautionary<br />

and preparedness actions regarding disasters. A content<br />

analysis will be conducted of messages from the National<br />

Weather Service, American Red Cross, and Federal Emergency<br />

Management Agency (FEMA) to understand how messages<br />

addressed the U in the CAUSE model, understanding, while<br />

discussing precautionary and preparedness actions regarding<br />

Hurricane Sandy.<br />

W2-E.4 Kponee, K*; Vorhees, D; Heiger-Benays, W; Boston<br />

University School of Public Health; kzkponee@bu.edu<br />

Exposure to highly contaminated drinking water in a<br />

rural Nigerian Village<br />

In 2011, the United Nations Environment Programme (UNEP)<br />

reported findings from its study of petroleum hydrocarbon<br />

contamination in the Ogoniland region of Nigeria where<br />

frequent and massive oil spills have destroyed vast areas,<br />

including farmland, fisheries, and water supplies. UNEP<br />

recommended research and remedial action to protect human<br />

health, with some recommendations focused on the Ogale<br />

community where residents rely on a drinking water supply<br />

contaminated with benzene and other petroleum hydrocarbons.<br />

Benzene alone has been detected at concentrations almost<br />

2,000 times higher than the USEPA drinking water standard.<br />

UNEP staff observed people using the contaminated drinking<br />

water and noted that its strong odor. Because such elevated<br />

exposures are likely to be associated with a range of acute and<br />

chronic effects, UNEP recommended emergency provision of<br />

clean drinking water, medical surveillance, and implementation<br />

of a prospective cohort study to investigate the effects of<br />

exposure to the contaminated drinking water in Ogale. There<br />

are no reports indicating that these recommendations have<br />

been implemented except <strong>for</strong> provision of clean water in late<br />

2011. This study implements some of UNEP’s recommendations<br />

<strong>for</strong> Ogale. Based on an investigator-administered questionnaire<br />

to one hundred households, this study provides a (1) detailed<br />

assessment of exposure to the contaminated water supply; and<br />

(2) a preliminary comparison of self-reported symptoms and<br />

health outcomes in the community served by the contaminated<br />

drinking water supply and a nearby comparable community<br />

served by a relatively clean drinking water supply. This study<br />

represents the logical next step to determine whether the<br />

extremely high levels of exposure in Ogale might be associated<br />

with acute and chronic adverse health effects. It also might<br />

improve understanding of how oil spills affect human health, a<br />

question that has eluded those investigating oil spills involving<br />

lower levels of exposure.<br />

P.91 Kroner, O*; Wullenweber, A; Willis, AM; Toxicology<br />

Excellence <strong>for</strong> <strong>Risk</strong> Assessment (TERA); Kroner@TERA.org<br />

Rethinking <strong>Risk</strong> Data: ITER 2.0<br />

For 17 years, the International Toxicity Estimates <strong>for</strong> <strong>Risk</strong><br />

(ITER) database (www.tera.org/iter) has been a centralized<br />

source of peer-reviewed chronic human health risk values. ITER<br />

is a free resource, providing access to international risk<br />

in<strong>for</strong>mation with a side-by-side comparison of risk values. Since<br />

1996, the database has grown to include over 700 chemicals<br />

and includes risk values derived by organizations from around<br />

the world. However, during this time the world has seen major<br />

advancements in computational processing power, database<br />

mining and programming, and user interface design. In short,<br />

we are learning to extract more knowledge from the available<br />

data. With an eye to the future, a series of stakeholder surveys<br />

have been conducted to evaluate how ITER and risk in<strong>for</strong>mation<br />

is currently used by risk assessors, and how ITER may be<br />

restructured to be most useful to meet the risk assessment<br />

community’s needs. Survey results indicated several areas <strong>for</strong><br />

improvement and have spurred a call <strong>for</strong> ITER 2.0, which is<br />

currently underway. The redesigned system will be built to<br />

support additional problem <strong>for</strong>mulations (NAS, 2009), offering<br />

flexibility to include additional data types such as acute values,<br />

occupational exposure levels, biomonitoring equivalents, and<br />

possibly ecological risk values. Possible user interface<br />

enhancements will allow <strong>for</strong> searching and sorting by chemical<br />

class, target endpoint, date of derivation, and uncertainty<br />

factors, and allow <strong>for</strong> cross chemical comparisons and<br />

meta-analyses. The development of ITER 2.0 based on user<br />

feedback will help organizations share their risk work, and help<br />

risk scientists navigate the available data to streamline public<br />

health protection ef<strong>for</strong>ts.<br />

P.90 Kugihara, N; Graduate School of Human Sciences, Osaka<br />

University; kugihara@hus.osaka-u.ac.jp<br />

Effects of changing frequency of heterogeneous stimuli<br />

over time on estimation of frequency<br />

Two kinds of stimuli (i.e. photos and words) were shown<br />

repeatedly to participants <strong>for</strong> twenty minutes. The photos have<br />

neutral emotional valence (e.g. spoon, tissue paper and dish)<br />

and words were nonsense syllables (e.g. nuse, heyo and rue).<br />

These stimuli were shown according to two types of schedules,<br />

HF (high frequency) and LF (low frequency). As <strong>for</strong> HF, the<br />

presented frequency increased rapidly and reached the peak<br />

(60 times per minute) in two minutes, then decreased<br />

gradually. As <strong>for</strong> LF, it increased gradually and reached the<br />

peak (6 times per minute) in two minutes, then decreased. HF<br />

and LF schedule were done at the same time. In one condition,<br />

stimuli of HF were words and LF were photos and in another<br />

condition HF were photos and LF were words. If a disaster<br />

occurs, mass media have a tendency to try to identify and<br />

pursue a target in charge of the tragic event. The frequency of<br />

the newspaper articles pursuing targets varies and fluctuates<br />

through time. Some researches indicate that transitions in<br />

scapegoats (targets) occur as time advances from persons or<br />

groups to society or to our culture. My past laboratory studies<br />

showed that these transitions were mainly caused by our<br />

memory bias. This means that the rare targets picked up by<br />

articles overestimated and also the degree of the<br />

overestimation increases as time advances. However, these<br />

experiment used same kinds of stimuli. There<strong>for</strong>e participants<br />

may fail to discriminate these stimuli. To avoid this problem the<br />

present study used heterogeneous stimuli (words and photos)<br />

as HF and LF. Results showed that perceived frequencies of LF<br />

were overestimated, and the subjective peak of LF appeared<br />

later than actual peak. On the other hand frequencies of HF<br />

were underestimated, and estimation peak nearly corresponded<br />

to presentation peak. These results indicate that even if<br />

presented stimuli were heterogeneous we have subjective<br />

biases of frequency and of its peak estimation.<br />

December 8-11, 2013 - Baltimore, MD

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!