12.07.2015 Views

The Syntax of Early English - Cryptm.org

The Syntax of Early English - Cryptm.org

The Syntax of Early English - Cryptm.org

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS
  • No tags were found...

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

218 <strong>The</strong> syntax <strong>of</strong> early <strong>English</strong>It is likely that this reanalysis took place initially in existing sentences <strong>of</strong> thetype It is bad (for) you to say things like that, and that the new constructionsin (9) are an effect <strong>of</strong> it. As usual, it is only through the emergence <strong>of</strong> whatwould earlier have been ungrammatical constructions that we can obtain evidencefor reanalysis.What has happened in the case <strong>of</strong> reanalysis shown in (10) is that the childon the basis <strong>of</strong> the grammatical rules available to her guesses at the structure(rule) <strong>of</strong> the grammar that has produced this particular construction. <strong>The</strong>child may deduce the rule that corresponds to the older structure, but she mayalso postulate an innovative structure (based on some other pattern) whichproduces the same surface structure but also some ‘wrong’ or certainly innovativeresults (the examples in (9)). This is what Andersen (1973) calls abduction,and – as we saw in chapter 1 – is important in language change becauseit is the only type <strong>of</strong> ‘logical inference which can introduce and create novelideas’ (McMahon 1994: 94). <strong>The</strong> notion <strong>of</strong> abduction is closely related to that<strong>of</strong> reanalysis as many analysts see it.<strong>The</strong> innovative structure will <strong>of</strong> course be based on certain features <strong>of</strong> theprimary data that the language learner is exposed to. <strong>The</strong>re must be a triggeringexperience, and it is here that the mechanism <strong>of</strong> analogy plays an importantrole. In fact, analogy can be regarded as an important factor not just inlanguage learning but in learning <strong>of</strong> all kinds (see Itkonen 1994, Holyoak andThagard 1995) because it is a way <strong>of</strong> patterning and interpreting the world.When applying it to cases <strong>of</strong> syntactic change during acquisition, as we aredoing here, it is useful to distinguish two subtypes: analogy may either extendan existing construction or it may create a new construction on the basis <strong>of</strong> agrammatical pattern used elsewhere (for more discussion <strong>of</strong> how this kind <strong>of</strong>analogy works, see H<strong>of</strong>stadter 1995: 200 ff.). It is this latter subtype that is veryclose to the concept <strong>of</strong> reanalysis used by many historical linguists. <strong>The</strong> ‘input’pattern that forms the basis for creative analogy in the case at hand is the fixedSV word order pattern. It makes it possible for the child to reanalyse the preverbalNP in (10a) as a subject rather than an indirect object, even if the relevantNP clearly has an oblique case form.It is likely that children <strong>of</strong>ten abduce through creative analogy during theacquisition process. Most abductions, however, do not stick, and get ironedout. New structures only get established if the same abduction process iscarried out by many speakers (and perhaps not just children but also adults).This generally seems to happen in cases where surface strings have becomeambiguous. In any case <strong>of</strong> syntactic innovation due to reanalysis <strong>of</strong> ambiguoussurface strings, the causes for the emerging ambiguity therefore have to beexplained for the account to be complete.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!