12.07.2015 Views

The Syntax of Early English - Cryptm.org

The Syntax of Early English - Cryptm.org

The Syntax of Early English - Cryptm.org

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS
  • No tags were found...

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

10 <strong>The</strong> syntax <strong>of</strong> early <strong>English</strong>probably because the Old <strong>English</strong> system <strong>of</strong> morphological case marking waslost. Since dative and genitive were the cases combining with adjectives, adjectivesceased to be case selectors. <strong>The</strong> change would then be a change in theways in which case could be assigned. <strong>The</strong> latter view is the more interestingone. It is certainly the one with the most general validity. Dative and genitivecases did not only cease to combine with adjectives; the dative and (object)genitive cases were lost generally, with the loss <strong>of</strong> morphological cases, as wewill see in chapter 3.In a parameter-setting model <strong>of</strong> acquisition and change, the task <strong>of</strong> thelearner is to decide, on the basis <strong>of</strong> the evidence in her language environment,how to fill in the values for the various parametric options allowed by UG.Choosing the values for the parameters for any particular language is the maintask <strong>of</strong> language acquisition. With respect to the changes in Case marking byadjectives discussed above, we could say that in the Old <strong>English</strong> period, thelanguage learner was able to incorporate lexical Cases in her grammar becausethe system <strong>of</strong> morphological case distinctions (in combination with someother properties) enabled her to learn a distinction between structural andlexical Cases. This is what, according to Roberts (1985), makes theTransparency Principle superfluous, since the nature <strong>of</strong> the acquisitionprocess is such that the optimal grammar will be chosen. Roberts (1985, 1993)also suggests an explicit parametric account <strong>of</strong> the history <strong>of</strong> the <strong>English</strong>modals. While subscribing to Lightfoot’s story <strong>of</strong> the history <strong>of</strong> modals asessentially a change in word-class resulting from the loss <strong>of</strong> specific main verbcharacteristics, Roberts shows that in addition, this categorial reanalysis interactswith and is furthered by other instances <strong>of</strong> grammar change, such aschanges in verb placement, and changes in the system <strong>of</strong> subject–verb agreement.This makes it clear that the historical fate <strong>of</strong> the <strong>English</strong> modals was notnecessarily shaped as a random accumulation <strong>of</strong> exception features, leadingto a change in category forced by the Transparency Principle. <strong>The</strong> changesaffecting the modal verbs interacted with other, independent changes.1.2.3 More on language acquisition and grammar change<strong>The</strong> general spirit <strong>of</strong> the Principles and Parameters approach to languageacquisition and grammar change should be clear by now: languagelearningchildren, on the basis <strong>of</strong> a richly structured innate UG, construct agrammar <strong>of</strong> their native language on the basis <strong>of</strong> the language they hear beingspoken around them. <strong>The</strong>re is no consensus in the literature on how childrenproceed to do this, and this lack <strong>of</strong> consensus makes itself felt in acquisitionorientedwork on grammar change. In the following subsections, we give a

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!