10.06.2013 Views

mass-communication-theory

mass-communication-theory

mass-communication-theory

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

152 Section 3 From Limited-Effects to Critical Cultural Theories: Ferment in the Field<br />

Examining the communicator, Hovland and his group studied the power of<br />

source credibility, which they divided into trustworthiness and expertness. As you<br />

might expect, they found that high-credibility communicators produced increased<br />

amounts of attitude change; low-credibility communicators produced less attitude<br />

change.<br />

Looking at the content of the <strong>communication</strong>, Hovland and his group examined<br />

two general aspects of content: the nature of the appeal itself and its organization.<br />

Focusing specifically on fear-arousing appeals, the Yale group tested the<br />

logical assumption that stronger, fear-arousing presentations will lead to greater<br />

attitude change. This relationship was found to be true to some extent, but variables<br />

such as the vividness of the threat’s description and the audience’s state of alarm,<br />

evaluation of the communicator, and already-held knowledge about the subject<br />

either mitigated or heightened attitude change.<br />

The Hovland group’s look at the organization of the arguments was a bit more<br />

straightforward. Should a communicator explicitly state an argument’s conclusions<br />

or leave them implicit? In general, the explicit statement of the argument’s conclusion<br />

is more effective, but not invariably. The trustworthiness of the communicator,<br />

the intelligence level of the audience, the nature of the issue at hand and its<br />

importance to the audience, and the initial level of agreement between audience<br />

and communicator all altered the persuasive power of a message.<br />

Regardless of how well a persuasive message is crafted, not all people are influenced<br />

by it to the same degree, so the Yale group assessed how audience attributes<br />

could mediate effects. Inquiry centered on the personal importance of the audience’s<br />

group memberships and individual personality differences among people<br />

that might increase or reduce their susceptibility to persuasion.<br />

Testing the power of what they called “counternorm <strong>communication</strong>s,”<br />

Hovland and his cohorts demonstrated that the more highly people value their membership<br />

in a group, the more closely their attitudes will conform to those of the<br />

group and, therefore, the more resistant they will be to changes in those attitudes.<br />

If you attend a Big Ten university and closely follow your school’s sports teams, it<br />

isn’t very likely that anyone will be able to persuade you that the Atlantic Coast<br />

Conference fields superior athletes. If you attend that same Big Ten university but<br />

care little about its sports programs, you might be a more likely target for opinion<br />

change, particularly if your team loses to an Atlantic Coast Conference team in a<br />

dramatic fashion.<br />

The question of individual differences in susceptibility to persuasion is not<br />

about a person’s willingness to be persuaded on a given issue. In persuasion<br />

research, individual differences refers to those personality attributes or factors that<br />

render someone generally susceptible to influence. Intelligence is a good example.<br />

It is easy to assume that those who are more intelligent would be less susceptible<br />

to persuasive arguments, but this isn’t the case. These people are more likely to<br />

be persuaded if the message they receive is from a credible source and based on<br />

solid logical arguments. Self-esteem, aggressiveness, and social withdrawal were<br />

several of the other individual characteristics the Yale group tested. But, as with<br />

intelligence, each failed to produce the straightforward, unambiguous relationship<br />

that might have seemed warranted based on commonsense expectations. Why?<br />

None of a person’s personality characteristics operates apart from his or her<br />

Copyright 2010 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s).<br />

Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!