10.06.2013 Views

mass-communication-theory

mass-communication-theory

mass-communication-theory

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

SOCIAL LEARNING<br />

Chapter 7 Moving Beyond Limited Effects: Focus on Functionalism and Children 193<br />

pent-up aggression and hostility. In television’s early days, many people were anxious<br />

to rationalize their use of this attractive new medium.<br />

There was even early scientific evidence suggesting that catharsis was indeed at<br />

work. Seymour Feshbach (1961) demonstrated what he said was catharsis by insulting<br />

college-age men with “a number of unwarranted and extremely critical remarks”<br />

in an experimental setting and then having them watch either filmed<br />

aggression (a brutal prize fight) or a neutral film (on the spread of rumors). The<br />

men were then asked to evaluate the experiment and the insulting experimenter.<br />

Those who had seen the prize fight were less aggressive in their attitudes than those<br />

who had seen the other film.<br />

But, as F. Scott Andison wrote in 1977 after reviewing twenty years’ worth of<br />

scientific evidence, “We can conclude on the basis of the present data cumulation<br />

that television, as it is shown today, probably does stimulate a higher amount of<br />

aggression in individuals within society. Therefore, it seems reasonable to tentatively<br />

accept the ‘TV violence as a stimulant to aggression’ <strong>theory</strong> and to reject the<br />

…‘cathartic’ theories” (p. 323). Or as James D. Halloran (1964/65), then director<br />

of Britain’s Center for Mass Communication Research at the University of Leicester,<br />

more directly put it, catharsis is a “phony argument.”<br />

But Feshbach did demonstrate a reduction in aggression after viewing, and he<br />

obtained similar results in a 1971 study (Feshbach and Singer) conducted with<br />

funding from NBC. The research was undertaken in a group home for preadolescent<br />

boys. For six weeks, half of the boys were restricted to watching television<br />

programs with little or no violence while the other half was allowed to watch violent<br />

content. A variety of behavioral measures indicated that the boys viewing the<br />

violent programs were less aggressive. These results may not have been caused by<br />

catharsis, however. The boys who were placed in the nonviolent programming<br />

group may have been frustrated because they were not allowed to watch some of<br />

their favorite shows. Heightened frustration might account for their increased<br />

aggressiveness.<br />

What social scientists would eventually learn, however, is that certain presentations<br />

of mediated violence and aggression can reduce the likelihood of subsequent<br />

viewer aggression. But catharsis is not the reason. Rather, viewers learn that violence<br />

might not be appropriate in a given situation. Think about the first Feshbach<br />

study (1961). Maybe those who had seen the brutal boxing match, who had seen<br />

unnecessary pain inflicted on another human, simply said to themselves, “Aggression<br />

is not a good thing.” Their aggressive drive might not have been purged, but<br />

they might have simply learned that such treatment of another human is inappropriate.<br />

In other words, their inclination toward aggression (remember, they had<br />

been insulted) was inhibited by the information in the media presentation. This<br />

leads us to the <strong>theory</strong> that is generally accepted as most useful in understanding<br />

the influence of media violence on individuals—social cognitive <strong>theory</strong>.<br />

Humans learn from observation. There has been some question, however, about<br />

how much and what kinds of behaviors people learn from the media. This debate<br />

has been fueled, in part, by a definitional problem. No one questions whether<br />

Copyright 2010 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s).<br />

Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!