15.11.2012 Views

Forbidden Words: Taboo and the Censoring of Language

Forbidden Words: Taboo and the Censoring of Language

Forbidden Words: Taboo and the Censoring of Language

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

244 <strong>Forbidden</strong> <strong>Words</strong><br />

Raising gooseflesh: extra-linguistic evidence<br />

Obscenities . . . are fighting words, gross words, dirty words, words charged with<br />

power; <strong>the</strong>y are hurled like insults, heaped up to contaminate <strong>and</strong> defile, to incite or<br />

inflame, or just to let <strong>of</strong>f steam. They leap out before we can stop <strong>the</strong>m. They<br />

draw attention, <strong>the</strong>y get us into trouble. The emotion <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> obscenity proceed<br />

toge<strong>the</strong>r, as if fused, overriding cortical inhibitions in a quick, involuntary burst.<br />

(Morris 2000: 174)<br />

We have presented plenty <strong>of</strong> linguistic evidence for <strong>the</strong> emotional quality <strong>of</strong><br />

taboo expressions. Even across languages <strong>the</strong>y can contaminate o<strong>the</strong>r words,<br />

bringing down innocent expressions that just happen to sound similar. Moreover,<br />

taboo senses seem to have a saliency that will dominate <strong>and</strong> suppress<br />

o<strong>the</strong>r senses <strong>of</strong> a language expression recruited as a euphemism. In this next<br />

section, we examine <strong>the</strong> psychological, physiological <strong>and</strong> neurological studies<br />

that all corroborate <strong>the</strong> fact that forbidden words are more arousing, more<br />

shocking, more memorable <strong>and</strong> more evocative than all o<strong>the</strong>r language<br />

stimuli. 14<br />

In 1957, Charles Osgood <strong>and</strong> his colleagues outlined a technique, <strong>the</strong><br />

‘semantic differential’, for systematically (though subjectively) quantifying<br />

connotative meaning; <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> results provide psycholinguistic evidence for<br />

<strong>the</strong> Allan–Burridge Law <strong>of</strong> Semantic Change <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> X-phemism mill. 15<br />

People were asked to evaluate words <strong>and</strong> phrases on a series <strong>of</strong> seven-point<br />

bipolar rating scales, intended to locate a concept in semantic space within<br />

three dimensions <strong>of</strong> attitude: evaluation (is <strong>the</strong> word good or bad?); activity<br />

(is <strong>the</strong> word active or passive?); potency (is <strong>the</strong> word strong or weak?). The<br />

research confirms what we know from <strong>the</strong> behaviour <strong>of</strong> words over time: that<br />

<strong>the</strong>re is a general tendency for any derogatory or unfavourable denotation or<br />

connotation within a language expression to dominate, whatever <strong>the</strong> context.<br />

A number <strong>of</strong> experiments measure <strong>the</strong> emotional impact <strong>of</strong> words using<br />

techniques such as electrodermal monitoring, which records skin conductance<br />

responses (galvanic skin responses). Research overwhelmingly supports what<br />

every native speaker knows: compared to o<strong>the</strong>r words, ‘dirty’ words cause<br />

bigger goosebumps; that is, <strong>the</strong>y evoke stronger skin conductance responses.<br />

16 There has been a lot <strong>of</strong> research into <strong>the</strong> effects <strong>of</strong> arousal on<br />

memory. The findings are always <strong>the</strong> same: taboo words, are more stimulating<br />

than non-taboo words <strong>and</strong> we appear to store <strong>the</strong>m differently in our<br />

memory. 17 Using natural conversation, researchers found excellent recognition<br />

memory for <strong>the</strong> surface structure <strong>and</strong> content <strong>of</strong> pr<strong>of</strong>ane <strong>and</strong> sexually<br />

suggestive language.<br />

The results showed that those 12 high interactional content sentences with pr<strong>of</strong>ane <strong>and</strong><br />

sexually suggestive language elicited responses quite different from those elicited by<br />

<strong>the</strong> remaining 44 high interactional content sentences . . . Sentences with <strong>of</strong>f-color

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!