25.12.2012 Views

Willard Van Orman Quine

Willard Van Orman Quine

Willard Van Orman Quine

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

136 peter hylton<br />

comments as follows:<br />

The intervening theory is composed of sentences associated with one another<br />

in multifarious ways not easily reconstructed even in conjecture.<br />

There are so-called logical connections, and there are so-called causal ones;<br />

but any such interconnections of sentences must finally be due to the conditioning<br />

of sentences as responses to sentences as stimuli. If some of the<br />

connections count more particularly as logical or causal, they do so only<br />

by reference to so-called logical or causal laws, which in turn are sentences<br />

within the theory. The theory as a whole – a chapter of chemistry, in this<br />

case, plus relevant adjuncts from logic and elsewhere – is a fabric of sentences<br />

variously associated to one another and to non-verbal stimuli by the<br />

mechanism of conditioned response. (WO 11)<br />

The meaning of ‘There is copper in it’ is thus by no means exhausted<br />

by its direct links to stimulation – by an account of which stimulations<br />

would prompt one to agree with it and which to disagree with<br />

it. The sentence also plays a role in our theory and is thus linked to<br />

countless other sentences of that theory.<br />

For our immediate purposes, the thing to emphasize about this<br />

account of the significance of language is that it all takes place at<br />

the level of sentences. It is observation sentences that are linked to<br />

stimulations; it is links between one sentence and others that play a<br />

role in the meaning of nonobservational sentences. We might put the<br />

point like this: An observation sentence is directly linked to stimulations,<br />

and those links determine its correct use and thus its meaning.<br />

For a nonobservation sentence, links to stimulations are equally important,<br />

but in this case those links are partly or wholly indirect. The<br />

sentence is linked to other sentences that are in turn linked to other<br />

sentences and so on, terminating in observation sentences. This terminus<br />

provides the empirical meaning for any sentence, however<br />

indirect and complex the connections may be. 24<br />

What we said about observation sentences may thus be generalized<br />

to all sentences, though with an important qualification. For any<br />

sentence, there is a sense in which what one needs to know to be a<br />

competent user of that sentence does not depend on the structure of<br />

the sentence – on its analysis into constituent parts. If one uses the<br />

sentence as a whole correctly, both in relation to stimulations and in<br />

relation to other sentences, then one is a competent user of it; that is<br />

all that can be required. In particular, it is not required that one see<br />

Cambridge Companions Online © Cambridge University Press, 2006

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!