25.10.2012 Views

Laurie Bauer - WordPress.com — Get a Free Blog Here

Laurie Bauer - WordPress.com — Get a Free Blog Here

Laurie Bauer - WordPress.com — Get a Free Blog Here

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

THE LINGUISTICS STUDENT’S HANDBOOK 90<br />

Elicitation<br />

Elicitation is asking speakers about their language. Despite the fact that this<br />

method of collecting data is virtually inescapable, and despite the fact that some<br />

linguists believe that speakers will have good intuitions about their own language,<br />

direct elicitation has to be treated with great caution. Problems may<br />

include things such as speakers not liking to contradict the linguist in any way<br />

because that would not be considered polite, speakers saying that something is<br />

impossible in their language when what they mean is that they cannot immediately<br />

think of a situation in which they might say it (linguists are far more<br />

used to thinking about sentences out of context than most speakers are), speakers<br />

be<strong>com</strong>ing confused because they are asked about too many similar structures<br />

at once, speakers being sidetracked by matters which are irrelevant for the<br />

linguist (e.g. Kim wouldn’t do that, but I’d believe it of Lee; I couldn’t say that,<br />

but it would be in order for a prince/woman/rude person to say it; we all know<br />

that happened last year, so you can’t use the present tense to describe it), and<br />

speakers not being able to put their intuitions about some usage into words. In<br />

some cases, speakers seem to be just plain wrong. Quirk & Svartvik (1966: 49)<br />

point out that in an experiment they carried out A keeps changed very when and<br />

Not if I have anything to do with it got very similar results for acceptability. For<br />

all these reasons, elicitation is best if carefully planned so that the informant or<br />

consultant is not inadvertently led to give a particular set of responses or<br />

exhausted in any particular session, and so that the elicitation uses behaviour<br />

which is as natural as possible. The other side of this particular coin is that<br />

some speakers, even if they cannot express what is going on in linguistic terms,<br />

may have very good intuitions about what is important in their language.<br />

For all these reasons, a number of techniques have been designed in an<br />

attempt to make data elicitation as accurate as possible, and indirect methods<br />

of elicitation are often to be preferred over direct ones. There are, of course,<br />

traps. If you ask a speaker of English ‘Do you say diaper or nappy’, they may<br />

give you the answer they think you are looking for (for example, you may be<br />

more likely to get diaper if you speak with an American accent), but if you show<br />

them a picture of a baby wearing the relevant garment and ask what the baby<br />

is wearing, they may give you an answer depending on whether they perceive<br />

the garment as being made of cloth or some artificial fabric. In principle, there<br />

is a distinction to be made between the situation of the linguist trying to elicit<br />

details of a language from a single speaker of that language, and the linguist<br />

trying to elicit details of a language from large numbers of speakers (where statistical<br />

techniques may be used to determine degrees of acceptability, for<br />

instance). In practice, both require similar care. For some discussion of these<br />

matters see Quirk & Svartvik (1966), Greenbaum & Quirk (1970), and<br />

Newman & Ratliff (2001).

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!