12.12.2012 Views

Educational Psychology—Limitations and Possibilities

Educational Psychology—Limitations and Possibilities

Educational Psychology—Limitations and Possibilities

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

22 The Praeger H<strong>and</strong>book of Education <strong>and</strong> Psychology<br />

thing-in-itself. As a process this individual-context interaction results more in an ever-changing<br />

mutual modification than an act of producing a “finalized something.” Thus, individual <strong>and</strong><br />

context are coconstructed, as they enter into a dynamic interactive process—the human being<br />

changes as does the environment in which he or she operates. Jeanette Bopry in her chapter here<br />

clarifies our underst<strong>and</strong>ing of this coconstructivism as she describes perceptions as emerging from<br />

the interaction of a cognitive system with its environment. This interaction in the language of<br />

complexity theory is labeled “structural coupling.” Such a process, Bopry maintains, is recursive<br />

“in that changes in A triggered by B will trigger changes in B which will trigger changes in A.”<br />

Tara Fenwick in her chapter draws upon her own important work in complexity theory to highlight<br />

these insights. The systems shaped by the structural coupling, she maintains, are inseparable as<br />

they create “a new transcendent unity of action <strong>and</strong> identities.” Such insights hold profound<br />

implications for the future of educational psychology <strong>and</strong> pedagogy.<br />

For example, the field of neuroscience, John Weaver writes in his chapter on “Neuropolitics,”<br />

illustrates the biological <strong>and</strong> cognitive importance of structural coupling of the individual <strong>and</strong><br />

the environment. Every neuron in the brain is constructed to engage in a particular activity. Yet,<br />

at birth, Weaver contends, all neurons can be employed to perform any task regardless of their<br />

predisposition. Thus, human beings are capable of creating new neural networks to facilitate<br />

their insight into the surrounding cosmos. <strong>Educational</strong> psychologists can make good use of this<br />

neuroscientific underst<strong>and</strong>ing to help teachers <strong>and</strong> students create new neural matrixes by exposing<br />

them to new <strong>and</strong> diverse ways of seeing the world. In many ways this is an amazing<br />

scientific insight in that it subverts mechanistic forms of cognitive essentialism that insist humans<br />

cannot “learn intelligence,” that they cannot teach themselves to become smarter. Thus, structural<br />

couplings connecting students with diverse contexts <strong>and</strong> sociocultural processes produce neurological,<br />

cognitive, political, <strong>and</strong> ethical benefits. Critical interpretivists use this knowledge in their<br />

larger effort to reconceptualize educational psychology, in the process creating a psychology <strong>and</strong><br />

subsequently a pedagogy of optimism <strong>and</strong> hope.<br />

Thus, this educational psychology of optimism <strong>and</strong> hope focuses on the importance of these<br />

insights into the interaction of individual <strong>and</strong> context, the macro <strong>and</strong> the micro. As David<br />

Hung, Jeanette Bopry, Chee Kit Looi, <strong>and</strong> Thiam Seng Koh maintain in their chapter, “Situated<br />

Cognition <strong>and</strong> Beyond: Martin Heidegger on Transformations in Being <strong>and</strong> Identity,” the whole<br />

is not made up of discrete things-in-themselves but is an interaction of intimately connected<br />

dynamics. The relationship connecting these entities, Hung, Bopry, Looi, <strong>and</strong> Koh posit, shapes<br />

the meanings they assume. No meaning exists outside of these interrelationships. Indeed, the<br />

mind is shaped by these structural couplings <strong>and</strong> cognitive activity comes to be understood in<br />

terms of this individual-contextual relationship <strong>and</strong> the coconstructive process that modifies both.<br />

Knowing in this configuration is always a social process seeking to interpret the meaning of<br />

diverse relationships. Teachers <strong>and</strong> learners in this complex process always know that there is no<br />

final interpretation. Epistemologically savvy, they realize that they must be humble for all of their<br />

interpretations are incomplete <strong>and</strong> flawed in ways not discernible in the present sociohistorical<br />

context.<br />

In this interpretivist context, learning, Tara Fenwick in her chapter reminds us, is viewed as<br />

a “continuous invention <strong>and</strong> exploration, produced through the relations among consciousness,<br />

identity, action <strong>and</strong> interaction, objects, <strong>and</strong> structural dynamics of complex systems.” Relationship<br />

in this domain takes on an importance previously unimagined in the psychological sciences.<br />

A quick return to some previously addressed concepts is appropriate in this context. Our previous<br />

discussion of epistemology <strong>and</strong> positivism’s unquestioned acceptance of a naïve realism becomes<br />

very important in this context. Intimately connected to the positivist epistemology is a positivist<br />

ontology that views the world as a simplistic domain composed of things-in-themselves that<br />

lend themselves to precise empirical measurement. Such an epistemology <strong>and</strong> ontology allow

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!