12.12.2012 Views

Educational Psychology—Limitations and Possibilities

Educational Psychology—Limitations and Possibilities

Educational Psychology—Limitations and Possibilities

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

652 The Praeger H<strong>and</strong>book of Education <strong>and</strong> Psychology<br />

continue to destroy) human creativity in the context of our relations with our social <strong>and</strong> natural<br />

environments.<br />

In colonial relations of power, hegemonic knowledges have allowed colonizers to secure<br />

their dominance through a fictional creation of sameness <strong>and</strong> commonality at the expense of<br />

difference <strong>and</strong> heterogeneity. To discuss therefore, the possibilities of educational change in North<br />

America we must first underst<strong>and</strong> the power of discursive interruptions to conventional practices<br />

of schooling that fail to account for difference in relation to ethnicity, gender, class, religion,<br />

language, <strong>and</strong> culture. Such identities are inextricably linked to schooling <strong>and</strong> to knowledge<br />

production. To underst<strong>and</strong> the nature <strong>and</strong> extent of colonial/colonized discourse <strong>and</strong> practice at<br />

school we must interrogate <strong>and</strong> hear the voices of different subjects as they speak about their<br />

schooling experiences. Colonialism when read as imposing <strong>and</strong> dominating never ceased with<br />

the return of political sovereignty to colonized peoples or nation states. Indeed, today colonialism<br />

<strong>and</strong> recolonizing projects are (re)produced in variegated ways. For example, within schools the<br />

manifestation of this process takes place in the different ways knowledges get produced <strong>and</strong><br />

receive validation, <strong>and</strong> the particular experiences of students who are counted as (in)valid in<br />

contrast to the identities of those that receive recognition <strong>and</strong> response from school authorities<br />

<strong>and</strong> discursive curricular practice. Through an examination of the power dynamics implicit in the<br />

evocation of culture, histories, knowledges, <strong>and</strong> experiences of the diverse bodies represented<br />

in the school system, we see how colonialism <strong>and</strong> colonial relations can be masked under the<br />

conventional processes of knowledge production <strong>and</strong> validation. In other words we are speaking<br />

of questions that seek answers to who counts, what counts, <strong>and</strong> why, in terms of different<br />

knowledges, multiple ways of knowing, identities, <strong>and</strong> experiences.<br />

EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY CRITIQUE AND BRIEF<br />

HISTORICAL OVERVIEW<br />

Historically, the field of mainstream educational psychology has viewed teaching <strong>and</strong> learning<br />

through a cultural lens that is predominantly Eurocentric in nature. Consequently, its conceptualization<br />

of how people learn, think, <strong>and</strong> develop ways of knowing in relation to their natural/social<br />

world, rests largely on post Enlightenment notions of deductive reasoning, cause <strong>and</strong> effect,<br />

stimulus/ response, <strong>and</strong> sensory/cognitive definitions <strong>and</strong> underst<strong>and</strong>ings of intelligence <strong>and</strong><br />

knowledge acquisition. Hermann Ebbinghaus (1850–1909), for example, argued that the cognitive<br />

connections a person makes between particular concepts is correlative to the frequency<br />

with which they are encountered (Driscoll, 1994). So for example the mental response a person<br />

may make to the stimulus of bread with the mental idea of butter will be governed by the<br />

number of times the person has experienced these two concepts in association with each other.<br />

In other words, learning <strong>and</strong> remembering is contingent upon frequency <strong>and</strong> repetition. Edward<br />

Thorndike (1874–1949)—the “father of educational psychology”—in pursuing the theory of<br />

stimulus/reflexive response, asserted that when a mental connection is made between a given<br />

situation <strong>and</strong> a response, the strength of the connection is increased as it is further used, practiced,<br />

or exercised (Joncich, 1962). Thus, the child who says “apple” at every sight of the fruit increases<br />

(according to Thorndike) his or her tendency to think <strong>and</strong> say apple at its every future appearance.<br />

The notion of stimulus <strong>and</strong> reflexive response with regard to learning became associated in<br />

turn with the idea that within all animal organisms a basic learning mechanism exists that can be<br />

conditioned by socio-environmental factors. The most notable example of this theory to be studied<br />

under experimental conditions can be found in the work undertaken by Ivan Pavlov (1849–1946).<br />

In his experiments with dogs Pavlov observed that after several experiences of hearing a tone just<br />

before food was placed in its mouth, the dog would begin to salivate in response to the tone even<br />

before it received any food (Driscoll, 1994). From then on the dog began to expect food when

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!