12.12.2012 Views

Educational Psychology—Limitations and Possibilities

Educational Psychology—Limitations and Possibilities

Educational Psychology—Limitations and Possibilities

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

26 The Praeger H<strong>and</strong>book of Education <strong>and</strong> Psychology<br />

awareness of selfhood. At times in the recent history of cognitive psychology—for example, in<br />

behaviorism—scientists insisted that consciousness did not exist because it did not lend itself<br />

to empirical measurement. Other cognitive perspectives, while not denying its existence three<br />

times before the cock crowed, simply ignored it. Obviously, such approaches to consciousness,<br />

immediate experience, <strong>and</strong> awareness of selfhood left an unfillable theoretical hole in its wake.<br />

Why, Varela asks, do humans experience the self so profoundly? Just ignoring the hole will not<br />

make it go away.<br />

Informed by enactivism we ask what is the nature of the disjunction between scientifically<br />

validated cognitive theory <strong>and</strong> our experience of consciousness. Operating on the grounding of<br />

our underst<strong>and</strong>ing of consciousness construction, we follow Varela’s description of the emergent<br />

<strong>and</strong> self-organizing dimensions of selfhood, his notion of the virtual self. The emergent, virtual<br />

self arises out of a maze of relationships—in much the same way hermeneutics describes the<br />

emergence of meaning in the relationships produced by the hermeneutic circle. It has no definable<br />

CPM, no “brain comm<strong>and</strong>” where control is coordinated. Consider this cognitive dynamic in<br />

light of our underst<strong>and</strong>ing of the cultural politics of the construction of the self. Such a process<br />

operates to create new social, cultural, political, <strong>and</strong> economic relationships to produce new <strong>and</strong><br />

more market-compliant, consumer selves. In this context we begin to underst<strong>and</strong> the pedagogical<br />

implications of the emergent self. The self is infinitely more malleable, more open to change than<br />

we had previously imagined. Given one’s motivation, of course, this dimension of selfhood can<br />

be mobilized for great benefit or manipulated for great harm.<br />

Buoyed by these insights, we enter the arena with a new insight into what can be. We know that<br />

despite the power of generations of cognitive determinists operating under the flag of IQ, human<br />

beings can learn to become more intelligent. Individuals can construct their own intelligence in<br />

a supportive context. And in this context such people underst<strong>and</strong> that selfhood is even more of a<br />

miraculous phenomenon than many had imagined. In the emergent context we gain a perspective;<br />

indeed, to live is to have a point of view. A critical teacher or researcher, however, gains numerous<br />

levels of underst<strong>and</strong>ing on the origins of his or her perspective.<br />

Varela writes of a moment-to-moment monitoring of the nature of our selfhood. Such monitoring<br />

involves gaining meta-awareness of the various connections we make to diverse dimensions<br />

of the sociophysical world around us. It involves isolating <strong>and</strong> letting go of an egocentrism that<br />

blinds us to the virtual <strong>and</strong> relational nature of our selfhood. In a critical interpretivist educational<br />

psychological context it means avoiding those definitions of higher-order thinking that view it<br />

as an egocentric manifestation of the combative proponent of rationality. In the process we also<br />

elude the cultural <strong>and</strong> gender inscriptions such perspectives drag along with them. With these<br />

knowledges we are prepared for the struggle to reconceptualize educational psychology.<br />

So critical interpretivists begin to play more focused attention to the ways complex systems<br />

display emergent properties by way of the interaction of simple elements. The structural couplings<br />

that develop in this interaction make possible such emergence. Thus, as Jeanette Bopry posits<br />

in her chapter on Varela, the human nervous system does not pick up information from the<br />

environment. Instead, it makes meaning, it interprets its interaction with its context. This is why<br />

enactivists assert that they don’t see the external environment but their own visual field. To figure<br />

out the significance of what they see in their fields, human beings—according to the enactivists—<br />

must reach out to others for help. How do my perceptions mesh with the perceptions of others?<br />

As Bopry puts it, “we share a reality because we have cospecified it through the coordination of<br />

our actions with the actions of others.” The development of a view of reality takes place in social<br />

interaction—such a view emerges from individuals talking to one another about what they see in<br />

their visual fields.<br />

In Western societies our language constructs a view of worldviews <strong>and</strong> knowledge about the<br />

world as a “thing” that one deposits in the container of the mind. Thus, knowledge is viewed as

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!