12.12.2012 Views

Educational Psychology—Limitations and Possibilities

Educational Psychology—Limitations and Possibilities

Educational Psychology—Limitations and Possibilities

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

406 The Praeger H<strong>and</strong>book of Education <strong>and</strong> Psychology<br />

be objective, but again we don’t know anything about genetically altered foods (Personal communication,<br />

November 10. 2000).<br />

The reader will note frequent use of the pronoun “we.” A common-sense underst<strong>and</strong>ing is that<br />

if the reporter did not know about an issue, then “we” as a society also did not know <strong>and</strong> the<br />

know-nothings include those people who may have a great deal of underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>and</strong> experience<br />

but since they are not part of the “we as reporters” group, are therefore irrelevant. Knowledge is<br />

again something based on one’s “nose for news” or “professional” ability to smell, hear, <strong>and</strong> see<br />

the truth. Knowledge is not that which comes from critical reviews of relevant literature, debate,<br />

or grappling with uncertainty, but from one’s direct experience in <strong>and</strong> of the world.<br />

From my interviews, it became clear that pre-1997 reporters had read nothing to very little<br />

about eating disorders beyond their peers’ accounts of the illnesses, yet they were confident that<br />

they were helping build knowledge <strong>and</strong> therefore were providing a public education function.<br />

When asked if there was enough evidence to write a piece about Montreux <strong>and</strong> its success,<br />

McLellan, who sees herself as building knowledge, expressed her opinion of the absurdity in<br />

waiting to tell what you know. “You could say that about any story—pollution—not enough<br />

evidence. You have to wait for the final results to see if separation is really good for Quebec. I<br />

think that there may have been positive stories written about her [Claude-Pierre] because she was<br />

doing something new <strong>and</strong> there was no success in hospitals either.” Again, McLellan, the healthreporter,<br />

is confident in explaining the absence of “no” success in hospitals <strong>and</strong> is thus setting up<br />

Montreux as an alternative in which common sense dictated that recovery would prevail.<br />

In 1999, ABC’s Goldberg explained to me that “If you press Peggy on the 100% she will<br />

back off on it. Her success rate is probably closer to 80% or 90%.” From his armchair research,<br />

however, Goldberg understood the failure rate in other facilities to be over 75%; thus his sense<br />

that the success rate at Montreux was 80% to 90% was extremely newsworthy. While in an<br />

interview in 2001, Goldberg seemed more cautious about providing a success rate but, he still<br />

was comfortable in relying on his senses to maintain his belief that Montreux, at least when he<br />

“discovered” it, had a “magic to it.” He was further convinced of Claude-Pierre’s success, given<br />

that she told him that no client had died at her facility. This seemed remarkable to Goldberg who<br />

had read that the mortality rate for people with anorexia was high. He was not aware that studies<br />

around mortality demonstrate that deaths generally did not occur in hospital or treatment centers<br />

where an anorexic was monitored, but rather when patients were out of hospital <strong>and</strong> at risk for<br />

complications caused by prolonged starvation or suicide (Crisp et al., 1992).<br />

Two of the women that 20/20 focused on as success stories have since died. After the death of<br />

Samantha Kendall, Goldberg stated she had left before treatment was completed <strong>and</strong> therefore<br />

could not be considered a Montreux failure because she had refused to finish the program. The<br />

second death was that of Donna Brooks. Goldberg believed that Montreux had in fact helped her.<br />

Seeing himself as witnessing Brook’s improvement, Goldberg focused on this past improvement<br />

rather than on the reality that she had died, weighing as little at death as she did before arriving<br />

at Montreux (Personal communication, July 10, 2001).<br />

Research, available at the time when 20/20 <strong>and</strong> other media were preparing stories about<br />

Montreux, strongly indicated that a person could not be considered to be “recovered” while still<br />

in a residential program, given it was an unnatural setting (Garfinkel, 1986). Goldberg, though,<br />

saw “recovery” <strong>and</strong> provided the visual proof of this to viewers. His interpretive framework did<br />

not enable him to see discrepant facts in his miracle cure framework. He later found out that a<br />

third patient portrayed on 20/20 required intensive care after leaving Montreux, but he stated that<br />

Montreux still had helped her, articulating his “knowledge” that maybe there are some who just<br />

can’t be helped. A lack of definitive information was taken by McLellan <strong>and</strong> Goldberg as either<br />

implicit support or at the least not explicit opposition.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!