12.12.2012 Views

Educational Psychology—Limitations and Possibilities

Educational Psychology—Limitations and Possibilities

Educational Psychology—Limitations and Possibilities

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Albert B<strong>and</strong>ura 51<br />

<strong>and</strong> changed her own behavior as a result. This is an example of learning that takes place through<br />

vicarious reinforcement.<br />

Another Freudian assumption popular with psychologists at the time of B<strong>and</strong>ura’s early Bobo<br />

doll experiments was that viewing violent or aggressive acts would have a draining effect that<br />

reduced aggression in the individual. This assumption was termed the catharsis effect. Both<br />

B<strong>and</strong>ura’s Bobo doll study <strong>and</strong> his studies with aggressive adolescent males disproved the assumptions<br />

of the catharsis effect. On the basis of these studies <strong>and</strong> others, B<strong>and</strong>ura developed a<br />

theory of observational learning <strong>and</strong> motivation that he termed social cognition theory.<br />

In social cognition theory, B<strong>and</strong>ura presents human behavior as being largely a product of<br />

direct <strong>and</strong> indirect learning. As discussed previously, direct learning (also referred to as trial <strong>and</strong><br />

error learning) is reinforced through the learner’s receipt of rewards or punishments. Indirect<br />

learning (also called vicarious learning <strong>and</strong> observational learning) occurs when the learner alters<br />

his or her behavior without receiving rewards or punishment. Recall again the example of the<br />

brother <strong>and</strong> sister on the shopping trip with their mother. Before she began imitating her brother’s<br />

begging, the sister had received no direct reinforcement for her behavior; she observed the brother<br />

beg <strong>and</strong> be rewarded, then she changed her behavior. For B<strong>and</strong>ura, observational learning had<br />

important advantages over trial <strong>and</strong> error learning. Whereas trial <strong>and</strong> error learning is risky <strong>and</strong><br />

time-consuming, observational learning saves the learner both time <strong>and</strong> risk by allowing him or<br />

her to learn from the successes <strong>and</strong> mistakes of others. For B<strong>and</strong>ura, humans have a great capacity<br />

for symbolism; we can retain socially modeled information in the form of mental images or verbal<br />

descriptions that serve as symbols for future behavior. Through social modeling, individuals can<br />

extend their learning by using symbols from the original modeled behavior to guide future rules<br />

for action. Returning once again to the example of the brother <strong>and</strong> sister on the shopping trip,<br />

B<strong>and</strong>ura argued that the sister will be able to apply her learning to different situations. For<br />

example, having retained the symbol of her brother receiving a reward for begging his mother,<br />

she might try begging her father or gr<strong>and</strong>parents for a desired toy. She might try begging her<br />

mother to allow her to spend the night at a friend’s house. In each case, the learner becomes able<br />

to apply his or her observational learning to new situations in ways that guide his or her future<br />

actions.<br />

Central to B<strong>and</strong>ura’s theory of social cognition is the term triadic reciprocal causation, which<br />

describes the simultaneous influences of thoughts, feelings, <strong>and</strong> the environment on human behavior.<br />

For B<strong>and</strong>ura, human behavior results from interactions between individual biological factors<br />

(e.g., cognitive capabilities), psychological factors (e.g., emotional states), <strong>and</strong> the environment.<br />

These factors influence <strong>and</strong> are, in turn, influenced by one another; the interactions among these<br />

biological, psychological, <strong>and</strong> environmental factors produce variations in human behavior. The<br />

results of reciprocal causation are that humans are at the same time producers of <strong>and</strong> products of<br />

their environment.<br />

For a practical example of triadic reciprocal causation, imagine that you <strong>and</strong> other college<br />

students are seated on the first day of class, waiting for your professor to arrive. As you wait,<br />

you join in small talk with the other students. The professor arrives; upon entering the room she<br />

makes eye contact <strong>and</strong> confidently announces that class will now begin. According to B<strong>and</strong>ura,<br />

the behaviors of the professor will be influenced by her emotional state (e.g., Is she excited about<br />

teaching the course? Does she believe herself to be an effective instructor?), her cognitions (e.g.,<br />

her initial thoughts about the course <strong>and</strong> students), <strong>and</strong> the classroom environment. Suppose that<br />

when the professor enters the classroom you <strong>and</strong> your classmates continue with your small talk<br />

<strong>and</strong> fail to acknowledge her entrance. Your actions might create a negative classroom environment<br />

for the professor to react to. On the other h<strong>and</strong>, you <strong>and</strong> your classmates might stop talking as<br />

the professor enters <strong>and</strong> focus your attention on her, indicating that you are ready to begin class.<br />

These two very different environmental responses on your part will interact with the professor’s

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!