12.12.2012 Views

Educational Psychology—Limitations and Possibilities

Educational Psychology—Limitations and Possibilities

Educational Psychology—Limitations and Possibilities

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

6 The Praeger H<strong>and</strong>book of Education <strong>and</strong> Psychology<br />

this domain. To function effectively in an informed <strong>and</strong> ethical way, educational psychologists<br />

must come to underst<strong>and</strong> the ways the knowledge they are taught to accept as true are shaped by<br />

dominant power interests <strong>and</strong> ideologies. Such forces move educational psychologists to produce<br />

knowledge <strong>and</strong> engage in activities that often reward the socially, politically, <strong>and</strong> economically<br />

privileged <strong>and</strong> punish the marginalized (see Richardson <strong>and</strong> Woolfolk [1994]). In this context<br />

Ray Horn <strong>and</strong> myself <strong>and</strong> the authors included in this book emphasize the need for educational<br />

psychologists to carefully examine what passes as reason <strong>and</strong> validated research in the mechanistic<br />

tradition, in the process asking in a critical sense whose interests this most modernist of<br />

sciences serves.<br />

In this context a central question of psychology/educational psychology emerges. How we<br />

answer it shapes the way in which we approach the field. How do humans represent <strong>and</strong> make<br />

meaning of the events that take place around them? Mechanistic psychologists maintain that<br />

the world is represented by symbols that are material (have substance) in some neuron-based or<br />

biochemical manner. In a more interpretive psychology the symbol processing that takes place<br />

is more conceptual <strong>and</strong> less biochemical. These symbols in interpretive psychology are very<br />

complex <strong>and</strong> cannot be separated from sociocultural <strong>and</strong> political contexts or situation-specific<br />

intentions, moods, <strong>and</strong> meaning constructions. In this context symbolic representation of the<br />

world <strong>and</strong> its events always connect the mind to micro (individualisitic) <strong>and</strong> macro (social)<br />

contexts. Thus, as we will emphasize throughout the h<strong>and</strong>book, educational psychology cannot<br />

be studied as simply an individualistic phenomenon.<br />

Making these distinctions in relation to the question about representation <strong>and</strong> meaning making,<br />

it is important to note that a central task of educational psychology involves developing a<br />

theory of learning. It is necessary but not sufficient for educational psychologists to possess a<br />

theory of representation <strong>and</strong> meaning making. The field has a more difficult task—to find out<br />

not how individuals learn but how they learn in particular sociocultural settings, e.g., school,<br />

work, leisure, etc. Such a task, interpretivists posit, cannot be accomplished by only studying<br />

quantitatively measured behavior of groups of individuals that can then be generalized universally.<br />

Instead, individuals must be studied in their natural settings (not labs) using a bricolage of<br />

research methods including ethnography, phenomenology, history, life history, semiotics, <strong>and</strong><br />

many others. Unfortunately, the most modern of sciences in its mechanistic articulation has<br />

not been comfortable using such research orientations. As a result, our underst<strong>and</strong>ing of how<br />

individuals represent <strong>and</strong> make meaning of the world <strong>and</strong> how they use these processes to learn<br />

about the world, themselves, <strong>and</strong> others has been profoundly compromised.<br />

BORN IN THE USA: MODERNITY, MECHANISM, AND REGULATION<br />

Thus, modern psychology <strong>and</strong> its educational psychological nephew were born in a Eurocentric,<br />

patriarchal, individuated, <strong>and</strong> decontextualized academic domain. The founding fathers within<br />

this mechanistic cosmos had faith that studying the abstracted, self-contained individual would<br />

lead them to an underst<strong>and</strong>ing of human life in general. Patricia Whang in her chapter in this<br />

volume extends this assertion contending that it is important to question “how the contributions<br />

made by educational psychologists have been constrained by the largely male <strong>and</strong> Euro-American<br />

perspectives, values, <strong>and</strong> traditions held by influential members of the field.”<br />

Since psychology emerged in movement from Western traditional to modern social orders, it<br />

was caught in the change of emphasis from the community <strong>and</strong> the household to the separate<br />

individual. In the premodern West, individuals were inseparable from the sociocultural context<br />

in which they were born <strong>and</strong> raised. Premodern westerners were simply not able to remove<br />

themselves from their social location <strong>and</strong> role(s) in order to try on new ways of being or new<br />

behaviors. To exist outside the local community was to “not be,” to cease to exist. One’s meaning

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!