08.09.2014 Views

Hazard anticipation of young novice drivers - SWOV

Hazard anticipation of young novice drivers - SWOV

Hazard anticipation of young novice drivers - SWOV

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

more difficulties to detect and recognize covert latent hazards than to detect<br />

and recognize overt latent hazards. The detection and recognition <strong>of</strong> a covert<br />

latent hazard requires the selection <strong>of</strong> a dominant schema that can predict<br />

the hazard on context aspects only. The possibility <strong>of</strong> an invisible other road<br />

user on collision course is something the driver has to infer from other visual<br />

cues (e.g. 'there can be something behind this lorry that blocks my view'). On<br />

the other hand, in case <strong>of</strong> overt latent hazards content aspects are visible. To<br />

put it differently: It is probably more difficult to expect things to happen<br />

without direct visible cues than to expect actions from others who are visible.<br />

As fixations on overt latent hazards not necessarily indicate that an overt<br />

latent hazard was recognized, only differences in mentioned overt latent and<br />

covert latent hazards in each group can provide information in support or<br />

not in support <strong>of</strong> the hypothesis. For both <strong>young</strong> learner <strong>drivers</strong> and older<br />

learner <strong>drivers</strong> the percentage <strong>of</strong> mentioned covert latent hazards was<br />

significantly lower than the percentage <strong>of</strong> mentioned overt latent hazards,<br />

but the difference in percentages mentioned overt latent hazards and<br />

mentioned covert latent hazards was not significant for experienced <strong>drivers</strong>.<br />

These results are in support <strong>of</strong> the hypothesis.<br />

Interestingly, the hazard detection and recognition task could be developed<br />

into a diagnostic tool for (learner) <strong>drivers</strong> to test their cognitive aspect <strong>of</strong><br />

hazard <strong>anticipation</strong>, without the use <strong>of</strong> an eye tracker. The scale <strong>of</strong><br />

mentioned latent hazards (overt and covert latent hazards combined) had an<br />

acceptable internal reliability, there was a large difference between<br />

experienced <strong>drivers</strong> and inexperienced driver and the relationship between<br />

fixated covert latent hazards and mention covert latent hazards was<br />

substantial. Jackson et al. (2009) used a slightly different task. In this task<br />

video clips were paused when overt latent hazards just became visible, but<br />

had not (yet) developed into an imminent threat. The screen turned black<br />

and participants had to tell what could happen next. Jackson et al. (2009) also<br />

found that experienced <strong>drivers</strong> mentioned significantly more potential<br />

hazards than inexperienced <strong>drivers</strong>. In the present study, participants<br />

watched short video clips in which overt latent hazards and covert latent<br />

hazards did not materialize. Directly after each clip participants were asked<br />

what could have happened that did not happen. Participants could also<br />

mention what drew their attention while they watched the video clip. An<br />

advantage <strong>of</strong> the method used in the present study is that participants are<br />

not triggered by what they have seen immediately before the video clip<br />

stopped. It could be that when the video stops after the first signs <strong>of</strong> the<br />

development <strong>of</strong> an overt latent hazard, this hazard was not recognized just<br />

169

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!