08.09.2014 Views

Hazard anticipation of young novice drivers - SWOV

Hazard anticipation of young novice drivers - SWOV

Hazard anticipation of young novice drivers - SWOV

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

"Driver distraction is a diversion <strong>of</strong> attention away from activities critical for<br />

safe driving toward a competing activity."<br />

Note that this definition <strong>of</strong> driver distraction excludes drowsy driving or<br />

driving without awareness when the workload is low and the driving task is<br />

monotonous (see Section 2.5.2). According to the definition distraction is<br />

about diverted attention and not about diminished attention (e.g. because <strong>of</strong><br />

fatigue or highway hypnoses). Note also that the definition includes poorly<br />

timed driving activities such turning on the wipers when immediate action is<br />

required to avert a crash. The competing activities in the definition can refer<br />

to interactions with equipment (both driving related and not driving related)<br />

in the vehicle, passengers, food, thoughts (<strong>of</strong> the driver her or himself) while<br />

the traffic situation is safety critical or developing into a safety critical<br />

situation. Being captured by objects, persons and events outside the vehicle<br />

not related to the safety <strong>of</strong> the traffic situation is also a 'competing activity'.<br />

One <strong>of</strong> the most remarkable results <strong>of</strong> the '100-Car Naturalistic driving<br />

study' (Dingus et al., 2006 see paragraph 2.5.2 for a description <strong>of</strong> this study)<br />

was that in nearly 80% <strong>of</strong> the crashes and 65% <strong>of</strong> the near-crashes distraction<br />

was a contributing cause. However, distraction in this study also included<br />

drowsy driving and 'non-specific eye glance away from the forward<br />

roadway'. These non-specific eye glances could be the result <strong>of</strong> internal<br />

distraction (e.g. when the driver is absorbed in her or his own thoughts), but<br />

could also be the effect <strong>of</strong> highway hypnoses. The first is included in the<br />

definition <strong>of</strong> distraction, but the latter not. When drowsy driving and nonspecific<br />

eye glances are excluded, still in nearly 68% <strong>of</strong> the crashes and 35%<br />

<strong>of</strong> the near-crashes distraction was a contributing factor. Some types <strong>of</strong><br />

distraction are more risky than others. Klauer et al. (2006) made a distinction<br />

between moderate secondary tasks and complex secondary tasks. Moderate<br />

secondary tasks were: talking and listing to a hand-held cell phone,<br />

inserting/retrieving a CD or cassette, reaching for a not moving object,<br />

combing or fixing hair, other personal hygiene (but not applying make-up),<br />

eating and looking at external objects. Complex secondary tasks were:<br />

dialling a number on a hand-held cell phone, locating/reaching/answering a<br />

hand-held device, operating a Personal Digital Assistant (PDA) or smart<br />

phone, reading, reaching for a moving object, insect in vehicle and applying<br />

make-up. On the basis <strong>of</strong> the data <strong>of</strong> the 100-Car Naturalistic driving study,<br />

Klauer et al. (2006) could calculate the likelihood <strong>of</strong> an at-fault crash or near<br />

crash when engaged in moderate secondary tasks or complex secondary<br />

tasks compared to non-distracted driving. The Odds Ratio (OR) for moderate<br />

secondary tasks was 2.10, 95% CI [1.62. 2.72]. The OR for complex secondary<br />

60

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!